Flavour as a psychological construct: implications for perceiving and measuring the sensory qualities of foods

Abstract While flavours comprised of simple taste/odour combinations can be easily decomposed, there is evidence that the individual components are seldom perceived independently. Manifestations of interactions include smell/taste confusions, attribution of taste properties such as sweetness or sourness to odours, and the enhancement and suppression of tastes by such odours. These phenomena are probably the result of blurring of the perceptual boundaries of odour and taste properties during repeated pairings as flavours. Following such pairings in the laboratory, odours can be shown to increase in perceived taste properties. It is proposed that during flavour formation, the components are encoded in memory in such a way that a later odour stimulus also elicits taste properties. Sensory properties such as sweetness can thus be seen as cognitive phenomena associated with both tastes and odours, in the latter case residing in memory. However, unlike some odour combinations, flavours, although usually perceived as a functional whole, are not indivisible synthetic entities. Perceptual interactions between odours and tastes are dependent on the extent to which an analytical approach is taken to the measurement of flavour qualities. Thus, odour enhancement of sucrose sweetness is not observed when the intensities of all the components in a flavour are rated. This has been interpreted as a consequence of rating strategies. However, the data are more consistent with a perceptual/cognitive interpretation. Recent research from our lab has shown that the cognitive strategy employed during the pairing of odours and tastes will also determine whether taste/odour interactions occur. The question of how to measure flavour properties is raised by these findings. Whether panellists focus on the sensory source of, for example, sweetness will depend upon the perceptual strategy they adopt. Asking panellists to focus only on the taste sweetness, while possible, may be assuming that odour and taste properties are independent within foods. ©

[1]  E T Rolls,et al.  Taste and olfactory processing in the brain and its relation to the control of eating. , 1997, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[2]  B. Slotnick,et al.  Flavor and the frontal cortex. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  E. Voirol,et al.  Comparative study of nasal and retronasal olfactory perception , 1986 .

[4]  Peeter W. J. Verlegh,et al.  The role of congruency and pleasantness in odor-induced taste enhancement. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[5]  Harry T. Lawless,et al.  Sensory science theory and applications in foods , 1991 .

[6]  R. Rescorla Simultaneous and successive associations in sensory preconditioning. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[7]  G. VonBékésy,et al.  OLFACTORY ANALOGUE TO DIRECTIONAL HEARING. , 1964, Journal of applied physiology.

[8]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Taste–smell interactions are tastant and odorant dependent , 1988 .

[9]  R. Boakes,et al.  Changes in Odor Sweetness Resulting from Implicit Learning of a Simultaneous Odor-Sweetness Association: An Example of Learned Synesthesia ☆ ☆☆ , 1998 .

[10]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[11]  Phil Reed,et al.  Compound stimulus preexposure effects in an appetitive conditioning procedure , 1995 .

[12]  D. Polzella,et al.  Smell and taste classification of the same stimuli. , 1995, The Journal of general psychology.

[13]  M. García-Medina Flavor-odor taste interactions in solutions of acetic acid and coffee , 1981 .

[14]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Strawberry odor, but not red color, enhances the sweetness of sucrose solutions , 1989 .

[15]  A. Noble,et al.  Time‐Intensity Evaluation of Sweetness and Fruitiness and Their Interaction in a Model Solution , 1990 .

[16]  C. Graaf,et al.  Sensory studies with sucrose-maltol mixtures , 1990 .

[17]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Flavor processing: more than the sum of its parts , 1997, Neuroreport.

[18]  John Prescott,et al.  The acquisition of taste properties by odors , 1995 .

[19]  W S Cain,et al.  Mutual action of taste and olfaction. , 1977, Sensory processes.

[20]  H. Lawless,et al.  Limiting response alternatives in time-intensity scaling: an examination of the halo-dumping effect. , 1994, Chemical senses.

[21]  Nicolette J. van der Klaauw,et al.  Scaling component intensities of complex stimuli: The influence of response alternatives , 1996 .

[22]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Both perceptual and conceptual factors influence taste-odor and taste-taste interactions , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  William S. Cain,et al.  Taste and olfaction: Independence vs interaction , 1980, Physiology & Behavior.

[24]  P. Rozin,et al.  “Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense , 1982, Perception & Psychophysics.

[25]  A. Dravnieks,et al.  Atlas of odor character profiles , 1992 .

[26]  J. Pierce,et al.  Orthonasal and retronasal odorant identification based upon vapor phase input from common substances. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[27]  E P Köster,et al.  Nasal, retronasal, and gustatory perception: An experimental comparison , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  E. Rolls,et al.  Gustatory, olfactory, and visual convergence within the primate orbitofrontal cortex , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.