Cognitive control during selection and repair in word production

ABSTRACT Word production entails selection of lexical items and their relevant segments among competitors, as well as monitoring and repair processes. In two experiments, we studied the control processes involved in selection (selection control) and covert-error- interception (post-monitoring control). Selection control was studied by manipulating the overlap (contextual similarity) either in semantics or in segments between two objects that participants repeatedly named. Post-monitoring control was examined by asking participants to switch the name of the two objects, thus suppressing a prepotent response (reversal). Results showed robust costs of contextual similarity (for both semantic and segmental overlap) and reversal, but the two did not interact. Moreover, we found no reliable correlation between the cost of contextual similarity at the semantic and segmental levels, suggesting stage-specific selection control processes. The reversal cost, however, was reliably correlated between semantically and segmentally related pairs, implying a control process that is shared by both stages of production.

[1]  Frank Oppermann,et al.  Resolving competition when naming an object in a multiple-object display , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Independence of Phonological and Orthographic Lexical Forms: Evidence from Aphasia , 1997 .

[3]  W. Levelt,et al.  Monitoring and self-repair in speech , 1983, Cognition.

[4]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[5]  Antje S Meyer,et al.  Activation of distractor names in the picture-picture interference paradigm , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[6]  J. Fiez Phonology, Semantics, and the Role of the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex , 2022 .

[7]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[8]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating Linguistic Processes in the Left Inferior Frontal Cortex with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2022 .

[9]  A. Meyer,et al.  Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  P. O'Seaghdha,et al.  The exception does not rule: attention constrains form preparation in word production. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Ardi Roelofs,et al.  Phonological Segments and Features as Planning Units in Speech Production , 1999 .

[14]  Jennifer E. Arnold,et al.  Title: the Effects of Anodal Stimulation of the Left Prefrontal Cortex on Sentence Production Conflict of Interest Statement Manuscript Accepted for Publication in Brain Stimulation , 2022 .

[15]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies , 2006, Cognition.

[16]  Albert Costa,et al.  Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access , 2005 .

[17]  A. Roelofs Tracing attention and the activation flow of spoken word planning using eye movements. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  Linda Wheeldon,et al.  Inhibitory form priming of spoken word production , 2003 .

[19]  A. Roelofs,et al.  Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. , 2003, Psychological review.

[20]  Gary S Dell,et al.  More on Lexical Bias: How Efficient Can a "Lexical Editor" Be? , 2009, Journal of memory and language.

[21]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Do perceived context pictures automatically activate their phonological code? , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[22]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Effect of name agreement on prefrontal activity during overt and covert picture naming , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  Brenda Rapp,et al.  Does segmental overlap help or hurt? Evidence from blocked cyclic naming in spoken and written production , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  M. Damian Articulatory duration in single-word speech production. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  R. Peereman,et al.  Do Phonological Codes Constrain the Selection of Orthographic Codes in Written Picture Naming , 2001 .

[26]  P. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Phonological competition and cooperation in form-related priming: sequential and nonsequential processes in word production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Desmond,et al.  The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Simon Fischer-Baum,et al.  Representation of Orthographic Knowledge , 2014 .

[29]  K. Rastle,et al.  Masked phonological priming effects in English: Are they real? Do they matter? , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Brenda Rapp,et al.  Mrs. Malaprop's Neighborhood: Using Word Errors to Reveal Neighborhood Structure. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.

[31]  Brenda Rapp,et al.  Modality and Morphology , 2015, Psychological science.

[32]  John C. Trueswell,et al.  Co-localization of Stroop and Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution in Broca's Area: Implications for the Neural Basis of Sentence Processing , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Clara D. Martin,et al.  Reconciling Phonological Neighborhood Effects in Speech Production through Single Trial Analysis Reconciling Phonological Neighborhood Effects in Speech Production through Single Trial Analysis , 2022 .

[34]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Consequences of an inhibition deficit for word production and comprehension: Evidence from the semantic blocking paradigm , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[35]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .

[36]  Sylvia A. A. Van Den Boogaard,et al.  Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in language production: Further evidence for the conceptual selection model of lexical access , 2004 .

[37]  M. Pickering,et al.  An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[38]  G. Dell,et al.  Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  R. Knight,et al.  Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in speech monitoring , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  M Coltheart,et al.  A position-sensitive stroop effect: Further evidence for a left-to-right component in print-to-speech conversion , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  Elizabeth R. Blacfkmer,et al.  Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech , 1991, Cognition.

[42]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Lexical selection in the semantically blocked cyclic naming task: the role of cognitive control and learning , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[43]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[44]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[45]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: a neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Elizabeth A. Hirshorn,et al.  Localizing interference during naming: Convergent neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence for the function of Broca's area , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Left Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Processing of Words and Sentences , 2016 .

[48]  A. Meyer,et al.  Single and multiple object naming in healthy ageing , 2007 .

[49]  H. Stadthagen-González,et al.  Abstract graphemic representations support preparation of handwritten responses , 2013 .

[50]  Sieb G. Nooteboom Monitoring for speech errors has different functions in inner and overt speech , 2010 .

[51]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[52]  G. Dell,et al.  The sequential cuing effect in speech production , 1994, Cognition.

[53]  Daniel Herron,et al.  A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. , 2004, Journal of memory and language.

[54]  Tatiana T. Schnur,et al.  The persistence of cumulative semantic interference during naming , 2014 .

[55]  Michael P. Sullivan,et al.  The Nature of Phonological Encoding During Spoken Word Retrieval , 1999 .

[56]  Gary M. Oppenheim,et al.  The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production , 2010, Cognition.

[57]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  A. Postma Detection of errors during speech production: a review of speech monitoring models , 2000, Cognition.

[60]  M. Schwartz,et al.  Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: Evidence from aphasia , 2006 .

[61]  W. Levelt,et al.  Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words , 2001, Cognition.

[62]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Autonomy of Lexical Orthography , 1997 .

[63]  Sharon L. Thompson-Schill,et al.  More attention when speaking: Does it help or does it hurt? , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  Markus F. Damian,et al.  Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm , 2003 .

[65]  Nazbanou Nozari,et al.  Did I say dog or cat? A study of semantic error detection and correction in children. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[66]  J. Kroll,et al.  Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .

[67]  G. Hickok Computational neuroanatomy of speech production , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[68]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[69]  G. S. Klein,et al.  SEMANTIC POWER MEASURED THROUGH THE INTERFERENCE OF WORDS WITH COLOR-NAMING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[70]  Robert J Hartsuiker,et al.  Semantic and phonological context effects in speech error repair. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[71]  G. Dell,et al.  Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.

[72]  Kevin Diependaele,et al.  How lingering representations of abandoned context words affect speech production. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[73]  G. Dell,et al.  Voxel-based lesion-parameter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a computational model of word production , 2013, Cognition.

[74]  S. Bookheimer Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[75]  S. Monsell,et al.  Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .

[76]  Martin Everaert,et al.  The linguistics enterprise : from knowledge of language to knowledge in linguistics , 2010 .

[77]  A. Meyer The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word ☆ , 1990 .

[78]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  Lexical selection is not by competition: Evidence from the blocked naming paradigm , 2014 .