The effects of increasing target prevalence on information processing during visual search
暂无分享,去创建一个
Kyle R. Cave | Nick Donnelly | Tamaryn Menneer | Hayward J. Godwin | K. Cave | H. Godwin | N. Donnelly | T. Menneer | Michael Thaibsyah | Michael Thaibsyah
[1] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. 26.5 brief comms NEW , 2005 .
[2] S. Mitroff,et al. Rare Targets Are Rarely Missed in Correctable Search , 2007, Psychological science.
[3] Jeremy M Wolfe,et al. Prevalence effects in newly trained airport checkpoint screeners: trained observers miss rare targets, too. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[4] S. Liversedge,et al. Saccadic eye movements and cognition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] Mark F McEntee,et al. The effect of abnormality-prevalence expectation on expert observer performance and visual search. , 2011, Radiology.
[6] R. Bakeman. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[7] Nick Donnelly,et al. High or low target prevalence increases the dual-target cost in visual search. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[8] Anina N. Rich,et al. Why do we miss rare targets? Exploring the boundaries of the low prevalence effect. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[9] K. Rayner. The 35th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[10] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .
[11] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Just Say No: How Are Visual Searches Terminated When There Is No Target Present? , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[12] J. Wolfe,et al. Varying Target Prevalence Reveals Two Dissociable Decision Criteria in Visual Search , 2010, Current Biology.
[13] Christopher P Benton,et al. The direction of measured face aftereffects. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[14] Evan M. Palmer,et al. Reaction time distributions constrain models of visual search , 2010, Vision Research.
[15] H. Field,et al. Investigation of the Climatic and Environmental Context of Hendra Virus Spillover Events 1994–2010 , 2011, PloS one.
[16] Andrew T. Smith,et al. Sensitivity of human visual cortical areas to the stereoscopic depth of a moving stimulus. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[17] Kyle R. Cave,et al. Dual-target search for high and low prevalence X-ray threat targets , 2010 .
[18] Nick Donnelly,et al. The impact of Relative Prevalence on dual-target search for threat items from airport X-ray screening. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[19] S. M. Luria,et al. Eye movements during search for coded and uncoded targets , 1975 .
[20] R. Walker,et al. A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[21] G. Zelinsky. A theory of eye movements during target acquisition. , 2008, Psychological review.
[22] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Even in correctable search, some types of rare targets are frequently missed , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[23] Naomi M. Kenner,et al. Low target prevalence is a stubborn source of errors in visual search tasks. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[24] Stefanie I. Becker. Determinants of Dwell Time in Visual Search: Similarity or Perceptual Difficulty? , 2011, PloS one.
[25] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .