Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Karl R Gegenfurtner | Thorsten Hansen | Lindsay T Sharpe | Emanuela de Luca | Herbert Jägle | K. Gegenfurtner | T. Hansen | L. Sharpe | H. Jägle | Emanuela de Luca
[1] A. Stockman,et al. The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype , 2000, Vision Research.
[2] David H. Brainard,et al. Calibration of a computer controlled color monitor , 1989 .
[3] Eberhart Zrenner,et al. The multifocal pattern electroretinogram (mfPERG) and cone-isolating stimuli , 2007, Visual Neuroscience.
[4] A. Stockman,et al. A luminous efficiency function, V*(lambda), for daylight adaptation. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[5] W. Andrew. The vertebrate visual system , 1957 .
[6] Heidi Hofer,et al. Organization of the Human Trichromatic Cone Mosaic , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[7] Paul G. Roofe,et al. The Vertebrate Visual System , 1958, Neurology.
[8] Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al. Color Vision: From Genes to Perception , 1999 .
[9] J. Nathans,et al. Opsin genes, cone photopigments, color vision, and color blindness , 1999 .
[10] G. H. Jacobs,et al. Functional consequences of the relative numbers of L and M cones. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[11] M. G. Nagle,et al. The tuning of human photopigments may minimize red—green chromatic signals in natural conditions , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[12] C. M. Cicerone,et al. The density of cones in the fovea centralis of the human dichromat , 1989, Vision Research.
[13] S. Mangel,et al. Colour thresholds in dichromats and normals , 2003, Vision Research.
[14] A. Wallace. The Colour Sense: its Origin and Development An Essay in Comparative Psychology , 1879, Nature.
[15] P. King-Smith,et al. Visual thresholds in dichromats and normals; the importance of Post-receptoral processes , 1981, Vision Research.
[16] N. Dominy,et al. Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates , 2001, Nature.
[17] C. F. Stromeyer,et al. Colour is what the eye sees best , 1993, Nature.
[18] B. B. Lee,et al. Sensitivity of macaque retinal ganglion cells and human observers to combined luminance and chromatic temporal modulation. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[19] Guy Verriest,et al. Spectral increment thresholds on a white background in different age groups of normal subjects and in acquired ocular diseases , 1977, Documenta Ophthalmologica.
[20] J Nathans,et al. Red, Green, and Red-Green Hybrid Pigments in the Human Retina: Correlations between Deduced Protein Sequences and Psychophysically Measured Spectral Sensitivities , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] G Wald,et al. Defective color vision and its inheritance. , 1966, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] Donald C Hood,et al. The multifocal electroretinogram (mfERG) and cone isolating stimuli: variation in L- and M-cone driven signals across the retina. , 2002, Journal of vision.
[23] A. Hendrickson,et al. Human photoreceptor topography , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[24] D H Kelly,et al. Two-band model of heterochromatic flicker. , 1977, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[25] M. Vorobyev,et al. Colour vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[26] J. Mollon,et al. Dichromats detect colour-camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[27] C. H. Graham,et al. SPECTRAL LUMINOSITY CURVES FOR PROTANOPIC, DEUTERANOPIC, AND NORMAL SUBJECTS. , 1957, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] C F Stromeyer,et al. Contributions of human long‐wave and middle‐wave cones to motion detection. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.
[29] J. Mollon. "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.
[30] Michael Bach,et al. Visual acuity and X-linked color blindness , 2006, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.
[31] H. Vries,et al. The heredity of the relative numbers of red and green receptors in the human eye , 1949, Genetica.
[32] Jay Neitz,et al. Estimates of L:M cone ratio from ERG flicker photometry and genetics. , 2002, Journal of vision.
[33] D. Fayuk,et al. The Journal of Physiology , 1978, Medical History.
[34] Jeremy Nathans,et al. Role of a locus control region in the mutually exclusive expression of human red and green cone pigment genes , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[35] J D Mollon,et al. Catarrhine photopigments are optimized for detecting targets against a foliage background. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.
[36] B. B. Lee,et al. Physiological mechanisms underlying psychophysical sensitivity to combined luminance and chromatic modulation. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[37] M. S. Loop,et al. Color vision sensitivity in normally dichromatic species and humans , 2004, Visual Neuroscience.
[38] Joel Pokorny,et al. Foveal cone detection statistics in color-normals and dichromats , 1991, Vision Research.
[39] Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al. Temporal and chromatic properties of motion mechanisms , 1995, Vision Research.
[40] Steven H. Schwartz,et al. Spectral sensitivity of dichromats: Role of postreceptoral processes , 1994, Vision Research.
[41] J. Neitz,et al. Flicker-photometric electroretinogram estimates of L:M cone photoreceptor ratio in men with photopigment spectra derived from genetics. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[42] B. C. Regan,et al. PII: S0042-6989(97)00462-8 , 1998 .
[43] Felix A. Wichmann,et al. The contribution of color to visual memory in X-chromosome-linked dichromats , 1998, Vision Research.
[44] A. Stockman,et al. Long-wavelength adaptation reveals slow, spectrally opponent inputs to the human luminance pathway. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[45] J. Kremers,et al. Cone signal contributions to electroretinograms [correction of electrograms] in dichromats and trichromats. , 1999, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.
[46] D. Norren,et al. Foveal cone mosaic and visual pigment density in dichromats. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.
[47] T Usui,et al. L/M cone ratios in human trichromats assessed by psychophysics, electroretinography, and retinal densitometry. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[48] H. Komatsu,et al. Dichromatism in macaque monkeys. , 1999, Nature.
[49] David Williams,et al. Functional photoreceptor loss revealed with adaptive optics: an alternate cause of color blindness. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[50] Jan Kremers,et al. Cone Signal Contributions to Electrograms in Dichromats and Trichromats , 2005 .
[51] Lindsay T Sharpe,et al. The molecular basis of dichromatic color vision in males with multiple red and green visual pigment genes. , 2002, Human molecular genetics.
[52] Daniel Osorio,et al. EVOLUTION AND FUNCTION OF ROUTINE TRICHROMATIC VISION IN PRIMATES , 2003, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[53] C. M. Cicerone,et al. The relative numbers of long-wavelength-sensitive to middle-wavelength-sensitive cones in the human fovea centralis , 1989, Vision Research.
[54] J D Mollon,et al. Chromaticity as a signal of ripeness in fruits taken by primates. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.