Neo-Noncontinuity Theory

Publisher Summary This chapter discusses the neo-noncontinuity theory. Noncontinuity theory is a theory dealing with choice responses during discrimination learning. Specifically, it holds that the choices reflect hypotheses that S is testing. It is less well known that noncontinuity theory is today enjoying a revival. Indeed, it may fairly be characterized as the leading theory of discrimination learning by adult humans. Theory, then, is proceeding at two levels, one abstract, probabilistic, intended for a variety of problem-solving situations; the other qualitative in description, concerned with the specific processes in the specific task. The development along these lines has already produced some notable accomplishments and promises both application to a greater variety of discrimination tasks and a finer analysis of each task. The concomitant development of theory and of probes, such as the blank-trials probe, should yield a rigorous, detailed, yet comprehensive description of human discrimination learning.

[1]  I. Krechevsky,et al.  A study of the continuity of the problem-solving process. , 1938 .

[2]  M. Levine The size of the hypothesis set during discrimination learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[3]  M. Levine,et al.  The presolution paradox in discrimination learning. , 1968, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[4]  M. Levine,et al.  THE BLANK TRIALS LAW: THE EQUIVALENCE OF POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AND NONREINFORCEMENT. , 1964, Psychological review.

[5]  J. R. Erickson,et al.  All-or-none assumptions in concept identification: analysis of latency data. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  F. Restle A theory of discrimination learning. , 1955, Psychological review.

[7]  M. Levine Mediating processes in humans at the outset of discrimination learning. , 1963, Psychological review.

[8]  E J GREEN,et al.  A simplified model for stimulus discrimination. , 1958, Psychological review.

[9]  W. Estes,et al.  Application of a statistical model to simple discrimination learning in human subjects. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  R N HABER,et al.  EFFECTS OF CODING STRATEGY ON PERCEPTUAL MEMORY. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  I. Krechevsky,et al.  "Hypotheses" in rats. , 1932 .

[12]  G. Bower,et al.  Presolution dimensional shifts in concept identification: A test of the sampling with replacement axiom in all-or-none models ☆ , 1966 .

[13]  F. Restle,et al.  Memory in concept attainment: effects of giving several problems concurrently. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  I ROCK,et al.  The role of repetition in associative learning. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[15]  D. Justesen,et al.  CONCEPT IDENTIFICATION: THE EFFECTS OF VARYING LENGTH AND INFORMATIONAL COMPONENTS OF THE INTERTRIAL INTERVAL. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Process models and stochastic theories of simple concept formation , 1967 .

[17]  L. E. Bourne,et al.  Mathematical theory of concept identification. , 1959, Psychological review.

[18]  Learning several concept-identification problems concurrently: a test of the sampling-with-replacement assumption. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  K. Spence An experimental test of the continuity and non-continuity theories of discrimination learning. , 1945 .

[20]  M. Levine Hypothesis behavior by humans during discrimination learning. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  F RESTLE,et al.  Toward a quantitate description of learning set data. , 1958, Psychological review.

[22]  T. J. Tighe,et al.  Perceptual learning in the discrimination processes of children: an analysis of five variables in perceptual pretraining. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  M. Glanzer,et al.  THE VERBAL-LOOP HYPOTHESIS: CONVENTIONAL FIGURES. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[24]  Murray Glanzer,et al.  Systematic operations in solving concept problems: A parametric study of a class of problems. , 1963 .

[25]  Response-contingent intertrial intervals in concept identification. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  W. K. Estes,et al.  All-or-none processes in learning and retention. , 1964 .

[27]  C. L. Hull Simple qualitative discrimination learning. , 1950, Psychological review.

[28]  C. Bunderson,et al.  Effects of delay of informative feedback and length of postfeedback interval on concept identification. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  R. R. Bush,et al.  A model for stimulus generalization and discrimination. , 1951, Psychological review.

[30]  G. Bower,et al.  REVERSALS PRIOR TO SOLUTION IN CONCEPT IDENTIFICATION. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  F RESTLE,et al.  The selection of strategies in cue learning. , 1962, Psychological review.

[32]  M. Levine,et al.  A model of hypothesis behavior in discrimination learning set. , 1959, Psychological review.

[33]  H. Kendler,et al.  Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving. , 1962, Psychological review.

[34]  M. Levine,et al.  The none-to-all theorem of human discrimination learning. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  M. Levine The assumption concerning "wrongs" in Restle's model of strategies in cue learning. , 1963 .

[36]  W. Estes,et al.  Learning theory and the new "mental chemistry". , 1960, Psychological review.

[37]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Memory in concept identification , 1964 .