Killing Characters in Video Games Kills Memory for In-Game Ads

Evolutionary theory predicts that people attend to emotionally arousing cues at the expense of less arousing cues. Violence is one emotionally arousing cue that attracts attention away from less arousing cues located in the same visual environment. Previous research has shown that violent media content attracts attention at the expense of brands advertised during violent media content. We predicted that participants who played a video game violently would recall and recognize fewer brands than participants who played the same game nonviolently. In Study 1, participants (N = 154) drove cars in the game The Getaway while real brands appeared within the city. Half of the participants played the game violently (running over characters) and half of the participants played the game nonviolently (carefully avoiding characters). Violent players recalled and recognized fewer brands than did nonviolent players. In Study 2, participants (N = 102) drove cars in the game Grand Theft Auto: San Andreas while fictitious brands were advertised on their vehicles. Half of the participants played the game violently (running over characters) and half of the participants played the game nonviolently (carefully avoiding characters). Violent players were less likely to recognize advertised brands than were nonviolent players. Not enough participants recalled brands to test whether violent players recalled fewer brands than did nonviolent players. These results across both studies suggest that within-game violence reduces the effectiveness of product placement, such that brands advertised in violent video games are less likely to be remembered than brands advertised in nonviolent video games.

[1]  P. Vorderer,et al.  It’s okay to shoot a character: Moral disengagement in violent video games , 2010 .

[2]  M. Nelson Recall of Brand Placements in Computer/Video Games , 2002, Journal of Advertising Research.

[3]  Eui Jun Jeong,et al.  Brand Logo Placements in Violent Games , 2011 .

[4]  A. d’Astous,et al.  A Study of Factors Affecting Consumer Evaluations and Memory of Product Placements in Movies , 2000 .

[5]  Terence A. Shimp,et al.  An Information-Processing Perspective on Recent Advertising Literature , 2012 .

[6]  Galen Clavio,et al.  Communicating with consumers through video games: an analysis of brand development within the video gaming segment of the sports industry , 2009 .

[7]  Robin S. Edelstein,et al.  Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach , 2009 .

[8]  D. Roskos-Ewoldsen,et al.  The Effectiveness of Brand Placements in the Movies: Levels of Placements, Explicit and Implicit Memory, and Brand-Choice Behavior , 2007 .

[9]  Annie Lang,et al.  The limited capacity model of mediated message processing , 2000 .

[10]  Seung-Chul Yoo,et al.  Do Violent Video Games Impair The Effectiveness of In-Game Advertisements? The Impact of Gaming Environment on Brand Recall, Brand Attitude, and Purchase Intention , 2011, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[11]  Ulrich G. Hofmann,et al.  When Items Become Victims: Brand Memory in Violent and Nonviolent Games , 2008, ICEC.

[12]  Jon K. Maner,et al.  From evolved motives to everyday mentation: Evolution, goals, and cognition , 2004 .

[13]  B. Bushman,et al.  Immersed in violence: Presence mediates the effect of 3D violent video gameplay on angry feelings. , 2014 .

[14]  Gary L. Wells,et al.  Stimulus Sampling and Social Psychological Experimentation , 1999 .

[15]  B. Gibson,et al.  Conscious and nonconscious effects of product placement: Brand recall and active persuasion knowledge affect brand attitudes and brand self-identification differently. , 2014 .

[16]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Stalking the elusive "vividness" effect. , 1982 .

[17]  Sungwon Chung,et al.  Motivated Processing of Peripheral Advertising Information in Video Games , 2016, Commun. Res..

[18]  M. Mather Emotional Arousal and Memory Binding: An Object-Based Framework , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[19]  Johannes Breuer,et al.  Comparing Apples and Oranges? Evidence for Pace of Action as a Confound in Research on Digital Games and Aggression , 2015 .

[20]  B. Bushman,et al.  Do Sex and Violence Sell? A Meta-Analytic Review of the Effects of Sexual and Violent Media and Ad Content on Memory, Attitudes, and Buying Intentions. , 2015, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Evolutionary Social Psychology , 2010 .

[22]  W. Peng,et al.  Need Satisfaction Supportive Game Features as Motivational Determinants: An Experimental Study of a Self-Determination Theory Guided Exergame , 2012 .

[23]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Vividness can undermine the persuasiveness of messages. , 1993 .

[24]  D. Schacter Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .

[25]  Youjae Yi Cognitive and affective priming effects of the context for print advertisements , 1990 .

[26]  Michael A. Wiles,et al.  The worth of product placement in successful films : an event study analysis , 2009 .

[27]  B. Bushman,et al.  Violence and Sex in Television Programs Do Not Sell Products in Advertisements , 2005, Psychological science.

[28]  Karyn Riddle,et al.  A Theory of Vivid Media Violence , 2014 .

[29]  B. Bushman,et al.  Violent and sexual media impair second-language memory during encoding and retrieval , 2015 .

[30]  B. Clifford,et al.  Individual and situational factors in eyewitness testimony. , 1978 .

[31]  D. Buss,et al.  The Evolution of Aggression. , 2006 .