SMELL-S and SMELL-R: Olfactory tests not influenced by odor-specific insensitivity or prior olfactory experience

Significance Currently available smell testing methods can be confounded by the lack of prior experience or insensitivity to the odorants used in the test. This introduces a source of bias into clinical tests aimed at detecting patients with olfactory dysfunction. We have developed smell tests that use mixtures of 30 molecules that average out the variability in sensitivity to individual molecules. Because these mixtures have an unfamiliar smell and the tests are nonsemantic, their use eliminates differences in test performance due to familiarity with the smells or the words used to describe them. SMELL-S and SMELL-R facilitate smell testing in different populations, without the need to adapt test stimuli to account for differences in familiarity with the test odors. Smell dysfunction is a common and underdiagnosed medical condition that can have serious consequences. It is also an early biomarker of neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer’s disease, where olfactory deficits precede detectable memory loss. Clinical tests that evaluate the sense of smell face two major challenges. First, human sensitivity to individual odorants varies significantly, so test results may be unreliable in people with low sensitivity to a test odorant but an otherwise normal sense of smell. Second, prior familiarity with odor stimuli can bias smell test performance. We have developed nonsemantic tests for olfactory sensitivity (SMELL-S) and olfactory resolution (SMELL-R) that use mixtures of odorants that have unfamiliar smells. The tests can be self-administered by healthy individuals with minimal training and show high test–retest reliability. Because SMELL-S uses odor mixtures rather than a single molecule, odor-specific insensitivity is averaged out, and the test accurately distinguished people with normal and dysfunctional smell. SMELL-R is a discrimination test in which the difference between two stimulus mixtures can be altered stepwise. This is an advance over current discrimination tests, which ask subjects to discriminate monomolecular odorants whose difference in odor cannot be quantified. SMELL-R showed significantly less bias in scores between North American and Taiwanese subjects than conventional semantically based smell tests that need to be adapted to different languages and cultures. Based on these proof-of-principle results in healthy individuals, we predict that SMELL-S and SMELL-R will be broadly effective in diagnosing smell dysfunction.

[1]  M. D. Rabin Experience facilitates olfactory quality discrimination , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  T. Hummel,et al.  Ratings of overall olfactory function. , 2003, Chemical senses.

[3]  J. Albrecht,et al.  Correlation analyses of detection thresholds of four different odorants. , 2011, Rhinology.

[4]  Andreas Keller,et al.  Olfactory perception of chemically diverse molecules , 2016, BMC Neuroscience.

[5]  T. Hummel,et al.  The association between smoking and smell and taste impairment in the general population , 2008, Journal of neurology.

[6]  Thomas Hummel,et al.  Human olfactory lateralization requires trigeminal activation , 2014, NeuroImage.

[7]  R. Doty,et al.  Development of normative data for the Brazilian adaptation of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. , 2015, Chemical senses.

[8]  Ben-Chih Yuan,et al.  Cross-Cultural Application of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” Odor Identification Test , 2007, American journal of rhinology.

[9]  L. Johansson,et al.  Prevalence of Olfactory Dysfunction: The Skövde Population‐Based Study , 2004, The Laryngoscope.

[10]  M. Perola,et al.  Self‐Ratings of Olfactory Function Reflect Odor Annoyance Rather than Olfactory Acuity , 2008, The Laryngoscope.

[11]  N. Tommerup,et al.  Isolated and syndromic forms of congenital anosmia , 2012, Clinical genetics.

[12]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Olfactory deficits predict cognitive decline and Alzheimer dementia in an urban community , 2015, Neurology.

[13]  D. Malaspina,et al.  Hidden consequences of olfactory dysfunction: a patient report series , 2013, BMC Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders.

[14]  B. Coller,et al.  A Data‐Rich Recruitment Core to Support Translational Clinical Research , 2015, Clinical and translational science.

[15]  T. Hummel,et al.  Effects of olfactory training: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Rhinology.

[16]  R. Doty,et al.  Smell and taste disorders, a study of 750 patients from the University of Pennsylvania Smell and Taste Center. , 1991, Archives of otolaryngology--head & neck surgery.

[17]  R. Khan,et al.  Perceptual convergence of multi-component mixtures in olfaction implies an olfactory white , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  C. Rumeau,et al.  Olfactory exploration: State of the art. , 2016, European annals of otorhinolaryngology, head and neck diseases.

[19]  D. Hornung,et al.  Congenital Lack of Olfactory Ability , 1992, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[20]  L. Vosshall,et al.  An olfactory demography of a diverse metropolitan population , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Mainland,et al.  The prevalence of androstenone anosmia. , 2003, Chemical senses.

[22]  R. Doty,et al.  Olfactory dysfunction in parkinsonism , 1988, Neurology.

[23]  T. Hummel,et al.  Chemical complexity of odors increases reliability of olfactory threshold testing , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[24]  R. Doty Olfactory dysfunction and its measurement in the clinic and workplace , 2006, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[25]  T. Hummel,et al.  Is there directional smelling? , 1989, Experientia.

[26]  A. Keller,et al.  Humans Can Discriminate More than 1 Trillion Olfactory Stimuli , 2014, Science.

[27]  A. Holley,et al.  Odor discrimination and recognition memory as a , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  David A Bennett,et al.  Olfactory Impairment in Presymptomatic Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[29]  R. Doty,et al.  Prevalence and risk factors of taste and smell impairment in a nationwide representative sample of the US population: a cross-sectional study , 2016, BMJ Open.

[30]  R Brookmeyer,et al.  Projections of Alzheimer's disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. , 1998, American journal of public health.

[31]  B T Bates,et al.  The effects of sample size and variability on the correlation coefficient. , 1996, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[32]  R. Doty,et al.  Development of the university of pennsylvania smell identification test: A standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function , 1984, Physiology & Behavior.

[33]  P. Jurs,et al.  Intranasal trigeminal stimulation from odorous volatiles: Psychometric responses from anosmic and normal humans , 1978, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  R. Doty,et al.  Analysis of Position of Trial Sequence and Type of Diluent on the Detection Threshold for Phenyl Ethyl Alcohol Using a Single Staircase Method , 1996, Perceptual and motor skills.

[35]  M. Su,et al.  A Pilot Study of a Traditional Chinese Version of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test for Application in Taiwan , 2010, American journal of rhinology & allergy.

[36]  M. Laska,et al.  A comparison of the detection thresholds of odour mixtures and their components , 1991 .

[37]  Thomas Hummel,et al.  Olfactory disorders and their consequences for quality of life , 2005, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[38]  L. Klimek,et al.  Characteristics of olfactory disorders in relation to major causes of olfactory loss. , 2002, Archives of otolaryngology--head & neck surgery.

[39]  I. Croy,et al.  Comparison between odor thresholds for phenyl ethyl alcohol and butanol. , 2009, Chemical senses.

[40]  P. Mitchell,et al.  Validation of self-reported hearing loss. The Blue Mountains Hearing Study. , 2001, International journal of epidemiology.

[41]  R. Doty Intranasal trigeminal detection of chemical vapors by humans , 1975, Physiology & Behavior.

[42]  R. Doty Olfactory dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases: is there a common pathological substrate? , 2017, The Lancet Neurology.

[43]  R. Doty,et al.  Olfactory function in chemical workers exposed to acrylate and methacrylate vapors. , 1989, American journal of public health.

[44]  Marcus D. Ruopp,et al.  Youden Index and Optimal Cut‐Point Estimated from Observations Affected by a Lower Limit of Detection , 2008, Biometrical journal. Biometrische Zeitschrift.

[45]  G Kobal,et al.  "Sniffin' sticks": screening of olfactory performance. , 1996, Rhinology.

[46]  M. Wiesmann,et al.  Test-retest reliability of the olfactory detection threshold test of the Sniffin' sticks. , 2008, Chemical senses.

[47]  J. Amoore,et al.  Specific Anosmia: a Clue to the Olfactory Code , 1967, Nature.

[48]  R. Doty,et al.  Influence of intertrial interval and sniff-bottle volume on phenyl ethyl alcohol odor detection thresholds , 1986 .

[49]  I. Croy,et al.  Peripheral adaptive filtering in human olfaction? Three studies on prevalence and effects of olfactory training in specific anosmia in more than 1600 participants , 2015, Cortex.

[50]  T. Hummel,et al.  'Sniffin' sticks': olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. , 1997, Chemical senses.