Hierarchical control of different elbow-wrist coordination patterns

Abstract The present paper focused on the role of mechanical factors arising from the multijoint structure of the musculoskeletal system and their use in the control of different patterns of cyclical elbow-wrist movements. Across five levels of cycling frequency (from 0.45 Hz up to 3.05 Hz), three movement patterns were analyzed: (1) unidirectional, including rotations at the elbow and wrist in the same direction; (2) bidirectional, with rotation at the joints in opposite directions, and (3) free-wrist pattern, which is characterized by alternating flexions and extensions at the elbow with the wrist relaxed. Angular position of both joints and electromyographic activity of biceps, triceps, the wrist flexor, and the wrist extensor were recorded. It was demonstrated that control at the elbow was principally different from control at the wrist. Elbow control in all three patterns was similar to that typically observed during single-joint movements: elbow accelerations-decelerations resulted from alternating activity of the elbow flexor and extensor and were largely independent of wrist motion at all frequency plateaus. The elbow muscles were responsible not only for the elbow movement, but also for the generation of interactive torques that played an important role in wrist control. There were two types of interactive torques exerted at the wrist: inertial torque arising from elbow motion and restraining torque arising from physical limits imposed on wrist rotation. These interactive torques were the primary source of wrist motion, whereas the main function of wrist-muscle activity was to intervene with the interactive effects and to adjust the wrist movement to comply with the required coordination pattern. The unidirectional pattern was more in agreement with interactive effects than the bidirectional pattern, thus causing their differential difficulty at moderate cycle frequencies. When cycling frequency was further increased, both the unidirectional and bidirectional movements lost their individual features and acquired features of the free-wrist pattern. The deterioration of the controlled patterns at high cycling frequencies suggests a crucial role for proprioceptive information in wrist control. These results are suppportive of a hierachical organization of control with respect to elbow-wrist coordination, during which the functions of control at the elbow and wrist are principally different: the elbow muscles generate movement of the whole linkage and the wrist muscles produce corrections of the movement necessary to fulfill the task.

[1]  G. J. van Ingen Schenau,et al.  Force, velocity and energy flow during the overarm throw in female handball players. , 1985, Journal of biomechanics.

[2]  J. Soechting Elements of Coordinated ARM Movements in Three-Dimensional Space§ , 1989 .

[3]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[4]  Ronald F. Zernicke,et al.  Predictions for neural control based on limb dynamics , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  R. Zernicke,et al.  Biomechanics and developmental neuromotor control. , 1993, Child development.

[6]  F. Zajac,et al.  Determining Muscle's Force and Action in Multi‐Articular Movement , 1989, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[7]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D G Stuart,et al.  Animal solutions to problems of movement control: the role of proprioceptors. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  A. Gentile,et al.  Joint control strategies and hand trajectories in multijoint pointing movements. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  Z. Hasan,et al.  Timing and magnitude of electromyographic activity for two-joint arm movements in different directions. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  M. Hoy,et al.  The role of intersegmental dynamics during rapid limb oscillations. , 1986, Journal of biomechanics.

[13]  Z. Hasan,et al.  Initiation rules for planar, two-joint arm movements: agonist selection for movements throughout the work space. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  S. Swinnen Interlimb coordination : neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints , 1994 .

[15]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  D. I. Miller,et al.  Body segment contributions to sport skill performance: two contrasting approaches. , 1980, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[17]  Ym. Kots Fixed set of variants of interaction of the muscles of two joints used in the execution of simple voluntary movements , 1966 .

[18]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Organizing principles for voluntary movement: extending single-joint rules. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Path constraints on point-to-point arm movements in three-dimensional space , 1986, Neuroscience.

[20]  J. B. Haddow,et al.  A kinematic analysis of an upper extremity ballistic skill: the windmill pitch. , 1982, Canadian journal of applied sport sciences. Journal canadien des sciences appliquees au sport.

[21]  E. Thelen,et al.  Understanding movement control in infants through the analysis of limb intersegmental dynamics. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  M. E. Feltner Three-Dimensional Interactions in a Two-Segment Kinetic Chain. Part II: Application to the Throwing Arm in Baseball Pitching , 1989 .

[23]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[24]  Paolo Cavallari,et al.  Preferential coupling between voluntary movements of ipsilateral limbs , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  M. Hoy,et al.  Contrasting roles of inertial and muscle moments at knee and ankle during paw-shake response. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Latash,et al.  The relation between posture and movement: A study of a simple synergy in a two-joint task , 1995 .

[27]  M T Turvey,et al.  Dynamical aspects of learning an interlimb rhythmic movement pattern. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  R. F. Zernicke,et al.  Adaptive Dynamics of the Leg Movement Patterns of Human Infants: II. Treadmill stepping in Infants and Adults. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Egocentric and Allocentric Constraints in the Expression of Patterns of Interlimb Coordination , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  C. A. Putnam,et al.  Sequential motions of body segments in striking and throwing skills: descriptions and explanations. , 1993, Journal of biomechanics.

[32]  D A Hong,et al.  Task dependent patterns of muscle activation at the shoulder and elbow for unconstrained arm movements. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  S. J. Phillips,et al.  Quantification of intersegmental reactions during rapid swing motion. , 1983, Journal of biomechanics.

[34]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.

[35]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  C. A. Putnam,et al.  A segment interaction analysis of proximal-to-distal sequential segment motion patterns. , 1991, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[37]  D. Anton Occupational biomechanics , 1986 .