Reductionist and anti-reductionist perspectives on dynamics

In this paper, reduction and its pragmatics are discussed in light of the development in computer science of languages to describe processes. The design of higher-level description languages within computer science has had the aim of allowing for description of the dynamics of processes in the (physical) world on a higher level avoiding all (physical) details of these processes. The higher description levels developed have dramatically increased the complexity of applications that came within reach. The pragmatic attitude of a (scientific) practitioner in this area has become inherently anti-reductionist, but based on well-established reduction relations. The paper discusses how this perspective can be related to reduction in general, and to other domains where description of dynamics plays a main role, in particular, biological and cognitive domains.

[1]  Catholijn M. Jonker,et al.  Compositional Verification of Knowledge-Based Task Models and Problem-Solving Methods , 2003, Knowledge and Information Systems.

[2]  Gavan Lintern,et al.  Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997, Complex.

[3]  Catholijn M. Jonker,et al.  Analysis of the Dynamics of Reasoning Using Multiple Representations , 2002 .

[4]  John Bickle,et al.  Psychoneural Reduction: The New Wave , 1998 .

[5]  E. Kandel,et al.  Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning? , 1984, Psychological review.

[6]  Jan Treur,et al.  Putting intentions into cell biochemistry: an artificial intelligence perspective. , 2002, Journal of theoretical biology.

[7]  Frank Jackson,et al.  Functionalism and broad content , 1988 .

[8]  H. Westerhoff,et al.  Control theory of regulatory cascades. , 1991, Journal of theoretical biology.

[9]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[10]  Andrew S. Tanenbaum,et al.  Structured Computer Organization , 1976 .

[11]  Ian Sommerville,et al.  Requirements Engineering: Processes and Techniques , 1998 .

[12]  Jaegwon Kim Mind in a Physical World: An Essay on the Mind-Body Problem and Mental Causation , 2001 .

[13]  Philip Gasper,et al.  Reduction and Instrumentalism in Genetics , 1992, Philosophy of Science.

[14]  B. Palsson,et al.  Theory for the systemic definition of metabolic pathways and their use in interpreting metabolic function from a pathway-oriented perspective. , 2000, Journal of theoretical biology.

[15]  T. Gelder,et al.  Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition , 1995 .

[16]  S. Brison The Intentional Stance , 1989 .

[17]  Frank Jackson,et al.  Program explanation: a general perspective , 1990 .

[18]  Bjarne Stroustrup,et al.  C++ Programming Language , 1986, IEEE Softw..

[19]  Catholijn M. Jonker,et al.  Principles of component-based design of intelligent agents , 2002, Data Knowl. Eng..

[20]  H. Westerhoff,et al.  How to recognize monofunctional units in a metabolic system. , 1996, Journal of theoretical biology.

[21]  Catholijn M. Jonker,et al.  Agent-oriented modeling of the dynamics of complex biological processes I : single agent models , 2002 .

[22]  Catholijn M. Jonker,et al.  On the use of shared task models in knowledge acquistion, strategic user interaction and clarification agents , 2000, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[23]  Jan Treur,et al.  Temporal Semantics of Compositional Task Models and Problem Solving Methods , 1999, Data Knowl. Eng..

[24]  Arvinder Kaur,et al.  Component Based Software Engineering , 2010 .

[25]  David Thomas,et al.  The Art in Computer Programming , 2001 .

[26]  Ernest Nagel,et al.  The Structure of Science , 1962 .