The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior

The mammalian behavioral antipredator defense systems have been characterized in terms of reactions to present, localizable, threat stimuli: Analysis of the relationship between features of the predator and the environment, and specific defensive behaviors indicates that the latter can be predicted with a high degree of accuracy. A different set of defensive behaviors are seen when rats living in burrow systems are confronted by a predator outside the burrow. Flight to the burrow and immobility inside the burrow are followed by active investigation of the surface where the cat was seen (risk assessment), with all of these accompanied by inhibition of nondefensive behaviors such as eating, drinking, sexual behavior and aggression. Two additional behaviors are described in this context, ultrasonic cries made at a high initial rate (50% time) by animals inside the burrow systems, and declining over 1-2 hours following predator exposure, and, a specific modification of eating patterns when the subjects return to the surface and begin to eat. Eating bouts are shortened, with fewer episodes of continuous eating, interspersed with intervals of scanning of the environment, a vigilance activity previously reported in field research. This analysis of a broad spectrum of defensive behaviors is being used to develop test batteries providing simplified models of these phenomena.

[1]  H. Iwasawa,et al.  Diazepam and endorphin independently inhibit ultrasonic distress calls in rats. , 1987, European journal of pharmacology.

[2]  Vigilance and social organization in two species of primates , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  V. Cuomo,et al.  Ultrasonic vocalization in response to unavoidable aversive stimuli in rats: effects of benzodiazepines. , 1988, Life sciences.

[4]  R. Underwood Vigilance Behaviour in Grazing African Antelopes , 1982 .

[5]  Group size and activity in eastern grey kangaroos , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  A. Siegel,et al.  Forebrain structures regulating flight behavior in the cat , 1985, Brain Research Bulletin.

[7]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Fear and aggression in the rat , 1984 .

[8]  D. H. Owings,et al.  Snake‐directed Behavior by Snake Naive and Experienced California Ground Squirrels in a Simulated Burrow , 1978, Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie.

[9]  Vigilance Patterns in Humans , 1988 .

[10]  L. Dill,et al.  The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) , 1989 .

[11]  D. Benton Models of mother-infant interactions ― their use to screen for psychotropic activity , 1989 .

[12]  S. Kawashima,et al.  Ultrasonic vocalizations and aggressive behavior in male rats , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[13]  T. Tonoue,et al.  Inhibition of shock-elicited ultrasonic vocalization by opioid peptides in the rat: A psychotropic effect , 1986, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  D. Blanchard,et al.  The effects of ethanol and diazepam on reactions to predatory odors , 1990, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[15]  R. J. Barfield,et al.  The Role of Ultrasonic Vocalizations in the Regulation of Reproduction in Rats a , 1986, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Lawrence M. Dill,et al.  The Economics of Fleeing from Predators , 1986 .

[17]  J. Gray,et al.  The psychology of fear and stress , 1971 .

[18]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Behavioral correlates of chronic dominance-subordination relationships of male rats in a seminatural situation , 1990, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[19]  P. Moore,et al.  Foraging and Predation Risk in Yellow-Bellied Marmots , 1986 .

[20]  D C Blanchard,et al.  Defensive behavior of laboratory and wild Rattus norvegicus. , 1986, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  R. Letz,et al.  Ultrasound emission in infant rats as an indicant of arousal during appetitive learning and extinction. , 1977, Science.

[22]  A. Siegel,et al.  The organization of the hypothalamic pathways mediating affective defense behavior in the cat , 1985, Brain Research.

[23]  Jaak Panksepp,et al.  The neurochemical control of crying , 1980, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[24]  N. Caine Visual Scanning by Tamarins , 1984 .

[25]  D. A. Thomas,et al.  Analysis of ultrasonic vocalizations emitted by intruders during aggressive encounters among rats (Rattus norvegicus). , 1983, Journal of comparative psychology.

[26]  L. Goodman,et al.  The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics , 1941 .

[27]  David H. Barlow,et al.  Anxiety and its disorders : the nature and treatment of anxiety and panic , 1988 .

[28]  K. Larsson,et al.  Depression of postejaculatory ultrasonic vocalization by (+)-bicuculline , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[30]  R. Bell,et al.  The ontogeny of ultrasounds in two strains of Rattus norvegicus. , 1975, Experimental aging research.

[31]  SIZE-RELATED EFFECTS ON SEARCH TIMES IN NORTH AMERICAN GRASSLAND FEMALE UNGULATES' , 1988 .

[32]  R. J. Barfield,et al.  Role of the ultrasonic vocalization of the female rat (Rattus norvegicus) in sexual behavior. , 1987, Journal of comparative psychology.

[33]  A. Siegel,et al.  The role of the anterior hypothalamus in affective defense behavior elicited from the ventromedial hypothalamus of the cat , 1985, Brain Research.