Causal structures and counter-intuitive experiments in electricity

This study focuses on the similarities and differences in structure and meaning between pupils’ conceptions about steady state tasks and evolutionary tasks, in which the system under study undergoes changes over time. A nine‐item written questionnaire was given to 197 Greek secondary school pupils. Results showed that the majority of pupils employ causal structures for their predictions. Two models were identified: a ‘give’ model, applied by pupils in steady‐state tasks; and a ‘take’ model, applied in evolutionary tasks. Structural similarities and semantic differences were identified between these models. In the light of these results, the study also examined the types of experiments in introductory electricity that would or would not obtain a counter‐intuitive reaction in pupils.

[1]  Panagiotis Koumaras,et al.  Devons-nous utiliser des phénomènes évolutifs en introduction à l'étude de l'électricité ? Le cas de la résistance , 1994 .

[2]  Fred N. Finley,et al.  Variable uses of alternative conceptions: A case study in current electricity , 1992 .

[3]  Christoph von Rhöneck,et al.  A Study of Students' Understanding of Electricity in Five European Countries. , 1988 .

[4]  Bruce A. Sherwood,et al.  Electrical Interactions and the Atomic Structure of Matter: Adding Qualitative Reasoning to a Calculus-Based Electricity and Magnetism Course , 1993 .

[5]  Barbara Y. White,et al.  Mental Models and Understanding: A Problem for Science Education , 1992 .

[6]  J. Nussbaum,et al.  Alternative frameworks, conceptual conflict and accommodation: Toward a principled teaching strategy , 1982 .

[7]  L. Viennot Fundamental Patterns in Common Reasoning: examples in Physics , 1994 .

[8]  Jean Piaget,et al.  The child's conception of time; , 1969 .

[9]  Dimitris Psillos,et al.  Voltage presented as a primary concept in an introductory teaching sequence on DC circuits , 1988 .

[10]  W. Brewer Schemas versus mental models in human memory. , 1987 .

[11]  B. Andersson,et al.  The experiential gestalt of causation: a common core to pupils’ preconceptions in science , 1986 .

[12]  Keith J Holyoak,et al.  Pragmatic reasoning schemas , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Amos Dreyfus,et al.  Applying the cognitive conflict strategy for conceptual change - some implications, difficulties, and problems , 1990 .

[14]  Dimitris Psillos,et al.  Pupils’ Representations of Electric Current before, during and after Instruction on DC Circuits , 1987 .

[15]  Panagiotis Koumaras,et al.  Différenciation conceptuelle : un enseignement d'hydrostatique, fondé sur le développement et la contradiction des conceptions des élèves , 1995 .

[16]  Pieter Licht,et al.  Teaching electrical energy, voltage and current: an alternative approach , 1991 .

[17]  M. Linn,et al.  Learning and Instruction: An Examination of Four Research Perspectives in Science Education , 1988 .

[18]  Kathryn R. Wentzel,et al.  Social Competence at School: Relation Between Social Responsibility and Academic Achievement , 1991 .

[19]  William F. Brewer,et al.  The Role of Anomalous Data in Knowledge Acquisition: A Theoretical Framework and Implications for Science Instruction , 1993 .

[20]  Dimitris Psillos,et al.  Multiple Causal Modelling of Electrical Circuits for Enhancing Knowledge Intelligibility , 1993 .

[21]  A. Demetriou,et al.  Experiential Structuralism and Neo-Piagetian Theories Toward an Integrated Model , 1987 .

[22]  David Shipstone,et al.  Pupils' Understanding of Simple Electrical Circuits: Some Implications for Instruction. , 1988 .

[23]  James Evans,et al.  Teaching electricity with batteries and bulbs , 1978 .

[24]  R. Driver,et al.  Children's Ideas in Science , 1985 .