Absolute Distance Perception to Locations off the Ground Plane

Using vision, humans can accurately judge distances to locations on the ground surface up to distances of at least 20 m. Most theories of depth perception assume that this ability is associated with the fact that we live in a terrestrial world in which locations of interest often appear on the ground and for which feedback about distance is often available from nonvisual sources such as walking. Much less is known about the ability of humans to judge absolute distances to locations other than on or supported by the ground plane beyond a few meters, at which point binocular stereo provides at best limited information about distance scaling. We show that one commonly used action measure for probing absolute distance perception exhibits accurate performance, even for targets located on the ceiling of a large room. We follow this with evidence that distance to ceiling locations is recovered with a mechanism that depends, at least in part, on the angle from the line of sight to the target location and a gravity-based frame of reference.

[1]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.

[2]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon , 2001, Nature.

[3]  Frank H. Durgin,et al.  Distance Perception and the Visual Horizon in Head-Mounted Displays , 2005, TAP.

[4]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Indoor Environments , 2005, Perception.

[5]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Zheng Bian,et al.  The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Asymmetry in 3-D perceptual organization: Ground-like surface superior to ceiling-like surface , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  B. E. Dunn,et al.  Relative Height on the Picture-Plane and Depth Perception , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.

[10]  J. Loomis,et al.  Spatial updating of targets in front and behind , 2004 .

[11]  M M Cohen,et al.  Effect of structured visual environments on apparent eye level , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  W. Epstein,et al.  Perceived depth as a function of relative height under three background conditions. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information , 2004, Nature.

[15]  H A Sedgwick,et al.  Distance perception mediated through nested contact relations among surfaces , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[18]  H A Sedgwick,et al.  Distance perception across spatial discontinuities , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  M. Wraga,et al.  The role of eye height in perceiving affordances and object dimensions , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  M. Wraga Using eye height in different postures to scale the heights of objects. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.