Pre-coincidence brain activity predicts the perceptual outcome of streaming/bouncing motion display
暂无分享,去创建一个
Wenfeng Feng | Song Zhao | Chengzhi Feng | Yajie Wang | Lina Jia | Yu Liao | Wenfeng Feng | Yu Liao | Lina Jia | Chengzhi Feng | Song Zhao | Yajie Wang
[1] S. Shimojo,et al. Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plasticity and interactions , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[2] Gianluca Campana,et al. The origin of the audiovisual bounce inducing effect: A TMS study , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[3] Renaud Brochard,et al. Visual motion disambiguation by a subliminal sound , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[4] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[5] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[6] E Başar,et al. Multistable visual perception induces a slow positive EEG wave. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.
[7] Marina Schmid,et al. An Introduction To The Event Related Potential Technique , 2016 .
[8] G. Rees,et al. The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .
[9] G. Rees,et al. Predicting the Stream of Consciousness from Activity in Human Visual Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.
[10] Clara Casco,et al. Revealing the origin of the audiovisual bounce-inducing effect. , 2012, Seeing and perceiving.
[11] Michael Bach,et al. Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[12] J. Flanagan,et al. The Integrative Review. , 2018, International journal of nursing knowledge.
[13] W. Metzger. Beobachtungen über phänomenale Identität , 1934 .
[14] Philipp Sterzer,et al. A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[15] M. Raemaekers,et al. Widespread fMRI activity differences between perceptual states in visual rivalry are correlated with differences in observer biases , 2009, Brain Research.
[16] Charles Spence,et al. Bouncing or streaming? Exploring the influence of auditory cues on the interpretation of ambiguous visual motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] H. Jasper,et al. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[18] D. Heeger,et al. Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Picture the difference: electrophysiological investigations of picture processing in the two cerebral hemispheres , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[20] Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al. Spontaneous local variations in ongoing neural activity bias perceptual decisions , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[21] S A Hillyard,et al. P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[22] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Insights from Intermittent Binocular Rivalry and EEG , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[23] J. Connolly,et al. Assessment of working memory abilities using an event-related brain potential (ERP)-compatible digit span backward task , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[24] Yousuke Kawachi,et al. Auditory transients do not affect visual sensitivity in discriminating between objective streaming and bouncing events. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[25] Osvaldas Rukšėnas,et al. Reversal negativity and bistable stimuli: Attention, awareness, or something else? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[26] D. Lewkowicz,et al. Sound induces perceptual reorganization of an ambiguous motion display in human infants , 2003 .
[27] K. Fujii,et al. Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..
[28] Philip M Grove,et al. Disambiguating the Stream/Bounce Illusion With Inference. , 2016, Multisensory research.
[29] J. Polich,et al. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.
[30] Jennifer L. Campos,et al. Neural correlates of size illusions: an event-related potential study , 2009, Neuroreport.
[31] R. Sekuler,et al. Collisions between Moving Visual Targets: What Controls Alternative Ways of Seeing an Ambiguous Display? , 1999, Perception.
[32] Lauri Parkkonen,et al. Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[33] T W Picton,et al. The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[34] G. Rees,et al. Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.
[35] A Gevins,et al. Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.
[36] Sabine Kastner,et al. Neural correlates of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[37] Kenzo Sakurai,et al. Auditory Induced Bounce Perception Persists as the Probability of a Motion Reversal is Reduced , 2009, Perception.
[38] R. Blake. A neural theory of binocular rivalry. , 1989, Psychological review.
[39] Talma Hendler,et al. Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain , 2001, NeuroImage.
[40] S. Hillyard,et al. Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .
[41] B I Bertenthal,et al. Directional Bias in the Perception of Translating Patterns , 1993, Perception.
[42] P Ullsperger,et al. Amplitude variations in P300 component due to unpredictable stepwise change of stimulus probability. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[43] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[44] S Shimojo,et al. Postcoincidence trajectory duration affects motion event perception , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[45] Emanuel Donchin,et al. The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.
[46] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Stream/bounce event perception reveals a temporal limit of motion correspondence based on surface feature over space and time , 2011, i-Perception.
[47] Emad N Eskandar,et al. Parietal activity and the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[48] F. Attneave. Multistability in perception. , 1971, Scientific American.
[49] Clara Casco,et al. Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: attention alone does not explain why the discs are bouncing. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] E. Donchin,et al. On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[51] Ray Johnson. For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .
[52] S. Nishida,et al. Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[53] Michael Bach,et al. The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing , 2005, Vision Research.
[54] Yoshitaka Nakajima,et al. Audiovisual integration: an investigation of the "streaming-bouncing" phenomenon. , 2004, Journal of physiological anthropology and applied human science.
[55] W. Klimesch,et al. Visual P2 component is related to theta phase-locking , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.
[56] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Auditory event-related potentials associated with perceptual reversals of bistable pitch motion , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[57] Stephen A. Engel,et al. Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation , 2001, Nature.
[58] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of perceptual reversals for three different types of multistable images. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[59] N. Logothetis,et al. Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[60] Theodor Landis,et al. Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[61] Geraint Rees,et al. A Neural Basis for Percept Stabilization in Binocular Rivalry , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[62] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Early top-down influences on bistable perception revealed by event-related potentials , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[63] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[64] R. Verleger. On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.
[65] E. Donchin,et al. On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.
[66] Masaki Tomonaga,et al. Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[67] S. Shimojo,et al. When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.
[68] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[69] Timothy J. Andrews,et al. Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus Predicts Conscious Perception of Rubin's Vase–Face Illusion , 2002, NeuroImage.
[70] R. Heuser. Surprise, surprise , 2014, Catheterization and cardiovascular interventions : official journal of the Society for Cardiac Angiography & Interventions.
[71] S Shimojo,et al. Attentional Modulation in Perception of Visual Motion Events , 1998, Perception.
[72] Clara Casco,et al. Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: When sound congruence affects grouping in vision , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[73] Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al. Ongoing Activity Fluctuations in hMT+ Bias the Perception of Coherent Visual Motion , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[74] Randolph Blake,et al. Traveling waves of activity in primary visual cortex during binocular rivalry , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[75] M. Hallett,et al. Neural correlates of cross-modal binding , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[76] S. Luck,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.
[77] Andreas K. Engel,et al. Oscillatory Synchronization in Large-Scale Cortical Networks Predicts Perception , 2011, Neuron.
[78] R. Sekuler,et al. Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.
[79] R. Deichmann,et al. Eye-specific effects of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2005, Nature.
[80] Masahiro Takei,et al. Human resource development and visualization , 2009, J. Vis..
[81] Naohisa Ueda,et al. P1 and P2 components of human visual evoked potentials are modulated by depth perception of 3-dimensional images , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[82] E. Vogel,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .
[83] G. McCarthy,et al. Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.
[84] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Effects of the orientation of moving objects on the perception of streaming/bouncing motion displays , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[85] R. Johnson. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[86] Fang Fang,et al. Perceptual grouping and inverse fMRI activity patterns in human visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[87] J. Polich. Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .
[88] Sabine Windmann,et al. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Attentional Control over Bistable Vision , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.