Pre-coincidence brain activity predicts the perceptual outcome of streaming/bouncing motion display

When two identical visual discs move toward each other on a two-dimensional visual display, they can be perceived as either “streaming through” or “bouncing off” each other after their coincidence. Previous studies have observed a strong bias toward the streaming percept. Additionally, the incidence of the bouncing percept in this ambiguous display could be increased by various factors, such as a brief sound at the moment of coincidence and a momentary pause of the two discs. The streaming/bouncing bistable motion phenomenon has been studied intensively since its discovery. However, little is known regarding the neural basis underling the perceptual ambiguity in the classic version of the streaming/bouncing motion display. The present study investigated the neural basis of the perception disambiguating underling the processing of the streaming/bouncing bistable motion display using event-related potential (ERP) recordings. Surprisingly, the amplitude of frontal central P2 (220–260 ms) that was elicited by the moving discs ~200 ms before the coincidence of the two discs was observed to be predictive of subsequent streaming or bouncing percept. A larger P2 amplitude was observed for streaming percept than the bouncing percept. These findings suggest that the streaming/bouncing bistable perception may have been disambiguated unconsciously ~200 ms before the coincidence of the two discs.

[1]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Sensory modalities are not separate modalities: plasticity and interactions , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  The origin of the audiovisual bounce inducing effect: A TMS study , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Renaud Brochard,et al.  Visual motion disambiguation by a subliminal sound , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  Thomas C. Toppino,et al.  Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  E Başar,et al.  Multistable visual perception induces a slow positive EEG wave. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.

[7]  Marina Schmid,et al.  An Introduction To The Event Related Potential Technique , 2016 .

[8]  G. Rees,et al.  The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .

[9]  G. Rees,et al.  Predicting the Stream of Consciousness from Activity in Human Visual Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.

[10]  Clara Casco,et al.  Revealing the origin of the audiovisual bounce-inducing effect. , 2012, Seeing and perceiving.

[11]  Michael Bach,et al.  Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[12]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The Integrative Review. , 2018, International journal of nursing knowledge.

[13]  W. Metzger Beobachtungen über phänomenale Identität , 1934 .

[14]  Philipp Sterzer,et al.  A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  M. Raemaekers,et al.  Widespread fMRI activity differences between perceptual states in visual rivalry are correlated with differences in observer biases , 2009, Brain Research.

[16]  Charles Spence,et al.  Bouncing or streaming? Exploring the influence of auditory cues on the interpretation of ambiguous visual motion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[18]  D. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Picture the difference: electrophysiological investigations of picture processing in the two cerebral hemispheres , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al.  Spontaneous local variations in ongoing neural activity bias perceptual decisions , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  S A Hillyard,et al.  P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[22]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Insights from Intermittent Binocular Rivalry and EEG , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[23]  J. Connolly,et al.  Assessment of working memory abilities using an event-related brain potential (ERP)-compatible digit span backward task , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  Yousuke Kawachi,et al.  Auditory transients do not affect visual sensitivity in discriminating between objective streaming and bouncing events. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[25]  Osvaldas Rukšėnas,et al.  Reversal negativity and bistable stimuli: Attention, awareness, or something else? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  Sound induces perceptual reorganization of an ambiguous motion display in human infants , 2003 .

[27]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[28]  Philip M Grove,et al.  Disambiguating the Stream/Bounce Illusion With Inference. , 2016, Multisensory research.

[29]  J. Polich,et al.  Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[30]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Neural correlates of size illusions: an event-related potential study , 2009, Neuroreport.

[31]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Collisions between Moving Visual Targets: What Controls Alternative Ways of Seeing an Ambiguous Display? , 1999, Perception.

[32]  Lauri Parkkonen,et al.  Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[34]  G. Rees,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. , 1998, Science.

[35]  A Gevins,et al.  Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Sabine Kastner,et al.  Neural correlates of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Kenzo Sakurai,et al.  Auditory Induced Bounce Perception Persists as the Probability of a Motion Reversal is Reduced , 2009, Perception.

[38]  R. Blake A neural theory of binocular rivalry. , 1989, Psychological review.

[39]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Vase or face? A neural correlate of shape-selective grouping processes in the human brain , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .

[41]  B I Bertenthal,et al.  Directional Bias in the Perception of Translating Patterns , 1993, Perception.

[42]  P Ullsperger,et al.  Amplitude variations in P300 component due to unpredictable stepwise change of stimulus probability. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[43]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[44]  S Shimojo,et al.  Postcoincidence trajectory duration affects motion event perception , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[46]  Takahiro Kawabe,et al.  Stream/bounce event perception reveals a temporal limit of motion correspondence based on surface feature over space and time , 2011, i-Perception.

[47]  Emad N Eskandar,et al.  Parietal activity and the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent motion , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  F. Attneave Multistability in perception. , 1971, Scientific American.

[49]  Clara Casco,et al.  Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: attention alone does not explain why the discs are bouncing. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[51]  Ray Johnson For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .

[52]  S. Nishida,et al.  Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  Michael Bach,et al.  The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing , 2005, Vision Research.

[54]  Yoshitaka Nakajima,et al.  Audiovisual integration: an investigation of the "streaming-bouncing" phenomenon. , 2004, Journal of physiological anthropology and applied human science.

[55]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Visual P2 component is related to theta phase-locking , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[56]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials associated with perceptual reversals of bistable pitch motion , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[57]  Stephen A. Engel,et al.  Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation , 2001, Nature.

[58]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of perceptual reversals for three different types of multistable images. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[59]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Theodor Landis,et al.  Right parietal brain activity precedes perceptual alternation of bistable stimuli. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Geraint Rees,et al.  A Neural Basis for Percept Stabilization in Binocular Rivalry , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  Michael A. Pitts,et al.  Early top-down influences on bistable perception revealed by event-related potentials , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[63]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[64]  R. Verleger On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[65]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[66]  Masaki Tomonaga,et al.  Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[67]  S. Shimojo,et al.  When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.

[68]  Piotr Jaskowski,et al.  Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .

[69]  Timothy J. Andrews,et al.  Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus Predicts Conscious Perception of Rubin's Vase–Face Illusion , 2002, NeuroImage.

[70]  R. Heuser Surprise, surprise , 2014, Catheterization and cardiovascular interventions : official journal of the Society for Cardiac Angiography & Interventions.

[71]  S Shimojo,et al.  Attentional Modulation in Perception of Visual Motion Events , 1998, Perception.

[72]  Clara Casco,et al.  Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: When sound congruence affects grouping in vision , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[73]  Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al.  Ongoing Activity Fluctuations in hMT+ Bias the Perception of Coherent Visual Motion , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[74]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Traveling waves of activity in primary visual cortex during binocular rivalry , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[75]  M. Hallett,et al.  Neural correlates of cross-modal binding , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[76]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[77]  Andreas K. Engel,et al.  Oscillatory Synchronization in Large-Scale Cortical Networks Predicts Perception , 2011, Neuron.

[78]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.

[79]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Eye-specific effects of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2005, Nature.

[80]  Masahiro Takei,et al.  Human resource development and visualization , 2009, J. Vis..

[81]  Naohisa Ueda,et al.  P1 and P2 components of human visual evoked potentials are modulated by depth perception of 3-dimensional images , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[82]  E. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .

[83]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[84]  Takahiro Kawabe,et al.  Effects of the orientation of moving objects on the perception of streaming/bouncing motion displays , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[85]  R. Johnson A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[86]  Fang Fang,et al.  Perceptual grouping and inverse fMRI activity patterns in human visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[87]  J. Polich Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .

[88]  Sabine Windmann,et al.  Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Attentional Control over Bistable Vision , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.