A developmental investigation of ERP auditory rhyming effects

In a simple auditory rhyming paradigm requiring a button-press response (rhyme/nonrhyme) to the second word (target) of each spoken stimulus pair, both the early (P50, N120, P200, N240) and late (CNV, N400, P300) components of the ERP waveform evidenced considerable change from middle childhood to adulthood. In addition, behavioral accuracy and reaction time improved with increasing age. In contrast, the size, distribution and latency of each of several rhyming effects (including the posterior N400 rhyming effect, a left hemisphere anterior rhyming effect, and early rhyming effects on P50 latency, N120 latency and P200 amplitude) remained constant from age 7 to adulthood. These results indicate that the neurocognitive networks involved in processing auditory rhyme information, as indexed by the present task, are well established and have an adult-like organization at least by the age of 7.

[1]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children , 2020, Definitions.

[2]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[3]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  A. Sanford,et al.  Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. , 1974, Biological psychology.

[5]  N. E. Loveless,et al.  The impact of warning signal intensity on reaction time and components of the contingent negative variation , 1975, Biological Psychology.

[6]  A W Gaillard,et al.  The late CNV wave: preparation versus expectancy. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[7]  B. Byrne,et al.  Semantic and phonetic memory codes in beginning readers , 1979, Memory & cognition.

[8]  E. Donchin,et al.  Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials , 1980, Brain Research.

[9]  Ann M. Peters,et al.  Phonological encoding and ideographic reading by the disconnected right hemisphere: Two case studies , 1981, Brain and Language.

[10]  D. Shankweiler,et al.  Phonetic coding and order memory in relation to reading proficiency: A comparison of short-term memory for temporal and spatial order information , 1983, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[11]  D. Friedman,et al.  Cognitive brain potential components in adolescents. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[12]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks , 1984, Brain and Language.

[13]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Phonological Coding in Prereaders. , 1984 .

[14]  J. Das,et al.  Children's memory for phonemically confusable and nonconfusable letters: changes with age and reading ability. , 1984, Child development.

[15]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Exploring the Articulatory Loop , 1984 .

[16]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the phonological processing of words and non-words , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  C. Chiarello Hemisphere dynamics in lexical access: Automatic and controlled priming , 1985, Brain and Language.

[18]  Neil Reid Wide Range Achievement Test: 1984 Revised Edition , 1986 .

[19]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[20]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the interaction between orthographic and phonological information in a rhyme-judgment task , 1987, Brain and Language.

[21]  Derek Besner,et al.  Phonology, Lexical Access in Reading, and Articulatory Suppression: A Critical Review , 1987 .

[22]  W. Marslen-Wilson Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[23]  R. Wagner,et al.  The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. , 1987 .

[24]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[25]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Rhyme and Alliteration, Phoneme Detection, and Learning to Read. , 1990 .

[26]  S. Baumann,et al.  Neuromagnetic evidence of a dynamic excitation pattern generating the N100 auditory response. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  H. Neville,et al.  Language and , 2019, Adventure Diffusion.

[28]  T. Harmony,et al.  The late event related potentials CNV and PINV in normal and dyslexic subjects. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[29]  P. Huttenlocher Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children , 1991 .

[31]  H. Neville,et al.  Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, global probability, and stimulus sequence effects in children , 1992 .

[33]  J. Grafman,et al.  Distinctions and similarities among working memory processes: an event-related potential study. , 1992, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[34]  H. Neville,et al.  Visual and auditory sentence processing: A developmental analysis using event‐related brain potentials , 1992 .

[35]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly , 1992 .

[36]  Development of memory span for spoken words: The role of rehearsal and item identification processes , 1993 .

[37]  D F Stegeman,et al.  Phonological effects on the auditory N400 event-related brain potential. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  Motor and cognitive aspects of slow potentials , 1993 .

[39]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  "Probing" the nature of the CNV. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[40]  T. Yamada,et al.  AUDITORY EVENT‐RELATED POTENTIALS AND REACTION TIME IN CHILDREN EVALUATION OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT , 1993, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[41]  F. Bloom,et al.  Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  F. Rösler Beyond Reaction Time and Error Rate: Monitoring Mental Processes by Means of Slow Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1993 .

[43]  A. Monk,et al.  Learning to read: the role of short-term memory and phonological skills. , 1994, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[44]  Antje S. Meyer,et al.  Neurophysiological Manifestations of Phonological Processing: Latency Variation of a Negative ERP Component Timelocked to Phonological Mismatch , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  J. Connolly,et al.  Event-Related Potential Components Reflect Phonological and Semantic Processing of the Terminal Word of Spoken Sentences , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  G. Hitch,et al.  Articulatory loop and children's reading , 1994 .

[47]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Auditory and visual semantic priming using different stimulus onset asynchronies: an event-related brain potential study. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[48]  M. Coles,et al.  Event-related brain potentials: An introduction. , 1995 .

[49]  R. Knight,et al.  Role of prefrontal cortex in generation of the contingent negative variation. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  D. Lovrich,et al.  Late cognitive brain potentials, phonological and semantic classification of spoken words, and reading ability in children. , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[51]  R. Gillam,et al.  Phonological Awareness Training and Short‐Term Working Memory: Clinical Implications , 1996 .

[52]  R. Oades,et al.  Development and topography of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs): mismatch and processing negativity in individuals 8-22 years of age. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[53]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Slow negative brain potentials as reflections of specific modular resources of cognition , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[54]  P. Huttenlocher,et al.  Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[55]  C. Wood,et al.  Pre-school phonological awareness and subsequent literacy development , 1998 .

[56]  M. Besson,et al.  Semantic, repetition and rime priming between spoken words: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence , 1998, Biological Psychology.

[57]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Role of Parietal Cortex in Verbal Working Memory , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  S. E. Avons,et al.  Measures of phonological short-term memory and their relationship to vocabulary development , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[59]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Elicited during Phonological Processing Differentiate Subgroups of Reading Disabled Adolescents , 1998, Brain and Language.

[60]  D Bavelier,et al.  Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects: biological constraints and effects of experience. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  S. Gathercole Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  S. Lupker,et al.  Phonological and semantic priming: Evidence for task-independent effects , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[63]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  ERP effects of listening to speech: semantic ERP effects , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  M. Besson,et al.  Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study of Phonological Priming between Bisyllabic Spoken Words , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Phillip J. Holcomb,et al.  Phonological Processing in Visual Rhyming: A Developmental ERP Study , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.