Culture, executive function, and social understanding.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Masuo Koyasu | Seungmi Oh | Charlie Lewis | C. Lewis | M. Koyasu | Seungmi Oh | A. Ogawa | B. Short | Zhao Huang | Ayako Ogawa | Benjamin Short | Zhao Huang
[1] H. Wellman,et al. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.
[2] Finding your marbles: does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[3] J. Perner,et al. Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[4] L. Moses,et al. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.
[5] Youngihm Kwon,et al. Western Influences in Korean Preschool Education , 2002 .
[6] J. Carpendale,et al. Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[7] N. Freeman,et al. Social influences on false belief access: specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? , 1996, Child development.
[8] Stephanie M. Carlson,et al. How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory , 2002 .
[9] Vera Winter,et al. Sensory motor inhibition as a prerequisite for theory-of-mind: A comparison of clinical and normal preschoolers differing in sensory motor abilities , 2006 .
[10] L. French,et al. Developmentally appropriate teacher-directed approaches: Images from Korean kindergartens , 1998 .
[11] Jodie A. Baird,et al. Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind , 2005 .
[12] J. Perner,et al. How Parenting Style Affects False Belief Understanding , 2001 .
[13] Jianhong Wang,et al. Culture and the Kindergarten Curriculum in the People's Republic of China , 1996 .
[14] J. Farver,et al. Acculturation and Korean‐American Children’s Social and Play Behavior , 2000 .
[15] V. Slaughter,et al. Opening windows into the mind: mothers’ preferences for mental state explanations and children’s theory of mind , 2003 .
[16] P. Mitchell,et al. Siblings in the development of executive control and a theory of mind , 2000 .
[17] Josef Perner,et al. Theory of mind and self-control: more than a common problem of inhibition. , 2002, Child development.
[18] J. Astington,et al. The future of theory-of-mind research: understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. , 2001, Child development.
[19] Claire Hughes,et al. Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability , 1998 .
[20] Charissa S. L. Cheah,et al. Korean mothers’ proactive socialisation beliefs regarding preschoolers’ social skills , 2005 .
[21] C. Lewis,et al. Korean preschoolers' advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. , 2008, Child development.
[22] Josef Perner,et al. Theory of mind is contagious - you catch it from your sibs , 1994 .
[23] J. Dunn,et al. Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: individual differences and interrelations. , 1999, Child development.
[24] Stephanie M. Carlson,et al. The Development of Executive Functioning and Theory of Mind , 2006, Psychological science.
[25] Chris Pratt,et al. The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation , 1995 .
[26] Charles M. Super,et al. Asian and Euro-American parents’ ethnotheories of play and learning: Effects on preschool children’s home routines and school behaviour , 2004 .
[27] K. Kidd,et al. The world-wide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus , 1996, Human Genetics.
[28] Claire O'Malley,et al. A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. , 2004, Developmental science.
[29] L. Moses,et al. The role of inhibitory processes in young children's difficulties with deception and false belief. , 1998, Child development.
[30] M. Chandler,et al. False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind , 1995 .
[31] Douglas Frye,et al. Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning , 1995 .
[32] Mayumi Karasawa,et al. Komatsudani Then and Now: Continuity and Change in a Japanese Preschool , 2004 .
[33] Annette M. E. Henderson,et al. Executive Function and False-Belief Understanding in Preschool Children: Two Tasks Are Harder Than One , 2003 .
[34] Josef Perner,et al. Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions. , 2003, Child development.
[35] J. Bruner,et al. Cultural learning. Author's reply , 1993 .
[36] S. Leekam,et al. What are the Links Between Theory of Mind and Social Relations? Review, Reflections and New Directions for Studies of Typical and Atypical Development , 2004 .
[37] Penelope G. Vinden. Parenting attitudes and children's understanding of mind , 2001 .
[38] Philip David Zelazo,et al. Inhibition as a problem in the psychology of behavior , 2003 .
[39] C. Lewis,et al. CHINESE PRESCHOOLERS' FALSE BELIEF UNDERSTANDING: IS SOCIAL KNOWLEDGE UNDERPINNED BY PARENTAL STYLES, SOCIAL INTERACTIONS OR EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS? , 2006 .
[40] M. Bowerman,et al. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns , 1991, Cognition.
[41] M. Naito,et al. The development of false-belief understanding in Japanese children: Delay and difference? , 2006 .
[42] D R Olson,et al. The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.