Author ' s personal copy Decomposing the neural mechanisms of visual search through model-based analysis of fMRI : Top-down excitation , active ignoring and the use of saliency by the right TPJ
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A Treisman,et al. Feature binding, attention and object perception. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[2] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Inhibitory tagging in an interrupted visual search , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[3] R. Nicoll,et al. Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.
[4] D. Prince,et al. GABA A receptor-mediated currents in interneurons and pyramidal cells of rat visual cortex , 1998 .
[5] G. Deco,et al. Top-down selective visual attention: A neurodynamical approach , 2001 .
[6] Gustavo Deco,et al. Integrating fMRI and single-cell data of visual working memory , 2004, Neurocomputing.
[7] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.
[8] N. Logothetis,et al. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal , 2001, Nature.
[9] D. Bouwhuis,et al. Attention and performance X : control of language processes , 1986 .
[10] H. J. Muller,et al. SEarch via Recursive Rejection (SERR): A Connectionist Model of Visual Search , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.
[11] M. Seghier,et al. A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[12] Gustavo Deco,et al. Large-scale neural model for visual attention: integration of experimental single-cell and fMRI data. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.
[13] Antonio Torralba,et al. Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search. , 2006, Psychological review.
[14] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.
[15] C. Kennard,et al. The anatomy of visual neglect , 2003 .
[16] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Prioritization in visual search: Visual marking is not dependent on a mnemonic search , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[17] A. Mizuno,et al. A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..
[18] Susan L. Franzel,et al. Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Stephen Smith,et al. Prioritizing new over old: An fMRI study of the preview search task , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[20] F. Hamker. A dynamic model of how feature cues guide spatial attention , 2004, Vision Research.
[21] A. Treisman,et al. Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] M. Corbetta,et al. Top-Down Control of Human Visual Cortex by Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Anticipatory Visual Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[24] Jacqueline C Snow,et al. Stimulus- and goal-driven biases of selective attention following unilateral brain damage: implications for rehabilitation of spatial neglect and extinction. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.
[25] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] E. Rolls,et al. Neurodynamics of biased competition and cooperation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[27] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Suppressive effects in visual search: A neurocomputational analysis of preview search , 2007, Neurocomputing.
[28] S. Yantis,et al. Object continuity in apparent motion and attention. , 1994, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[29] Pierre Baldi,et al. A bottom-up model of spatial attention predicts human error patterns in rapid scene recognition. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[30] Sasa Kenjeres,et al. Visualization of turbulence structures reorganization in thermal convection subjected to external magnetic field , 2004, J. Vis..
[31] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[32] J. O'Doherty,et al. Model‐Based fMRI and Its Application to Reward Learning and Decision Making , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[33] N. Spruston,et al. Dendritic glutamate receptor channels in rat hippocampal CA3 and CA1 pyramidal neurons. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.
[34] C. Stevens,et al. Voltage dependence of NMDA-activated macroscopic conductances predicted by single-channel kinetics , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[35] R. Luce,et al. The Choice Axiom after Twenty Years , 1977 .
[36] Robert T. Knight,et al. Top-down Enhancement and Suppression of the Magnitude and Speed of Neural Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Leila Reddy,et al. Coding of visual objects in the ventral stream , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[38] C. Koch,et al. Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[39] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. , 1997, Psychological review.
[40] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[41] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: using time in visual selection , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[42] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Saliency, attention, and visual search: an information theoretic approach. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[44] T. Mexia,et al. Author ' s personal copy , 2009 .
[45] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking inhibits singleton capture , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[46] P. Matthews,et al. A neural marker of content-specific active ignoring. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[47] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Individual Choice Behavior , 1959 .
[48] R. Nicoll,et al. Mechanisms generating the time course of dual component excitatory synaptic currents recorded in hippocampal slices , 1990, Neuron.
[49] Martin Stetter,et al. Modeling the Link between Functional Imaging and Neuronal Activity: Synaptic Metabolic Demand and Spike Rates , 2002, NeuroImage.
[50] A. T. Smith,et al. Attentional suppression of activity in the human visual cortex , 2000, Neuroreport.
[51] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Featural guidance in conjunction search: the contrast between orientation and color. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] Jacqueline C Snow,et al. Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: how intact is the ipsilesional field? , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[53] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Model Based Analysis of fMRI-Data: Applying the sSoTS Framework to the Neural Basic of Preview Search , 2009, WAPCV.
[54] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Visual marking: Evidence for inhibition using a probe-dot detection paradigm , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[55] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. A neural system for human visual working memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[56] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Separating distractor rejection and target detection in posterior parietal cortex—an event-related fMRI study of visual marking , 2003, NeuroImage.
[57] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Comparing Segmentation by Time and by Motion in Visual Search: An fMRI Investigation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[58] John K. Tsotsos,et al. Attention in Cognitive Systems, 5th International Workshop on Attention in Cognitive Systems, WAPCV 2008, Fira, Santorini, Greece, May 12, 2008, Revised Selected Papers , 2009, WAPCV.
[59] G W Humphreys,et al. Driving attention with the top down: The relative contribution of target templates to the linear separability effect in the size dimension , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[60] N. Logothetis. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI , 2008, Nature.
[61] D. McCormick,et al. Comparative electrophysiology of pyramidal and sparsely spiny stellate neurons of the neocortex. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] Dietmar Heinke,et al. Simulating posterior parietal damage in a biologically plausible framework: Neuropsychological tests of the search over time and space model , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[63] Min-Shik Kim,et al. Perceptual grouping via spatial selection in a focused-attention task , 2001, Vision Research.
[64] G. Glover. Deconvolution of Impulse Response in Event-Related BOLD fMRI1 , 1999, NeuroImage.
[65] G. Humphreys,et al. An analysis of the time course of attention in preview search , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[66] Paul Antoine Salin,et al. Spontaneous GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory currents in adult rat somatosensory cortex. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[67] Olaf B. Paulson,et al. Parieto–Occipital Areas Involved in Efficient Filtering in Search: A Time Course Analysis of Visual Marking using Behavioural and Functional Imaging Procedures , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[68] G. Humphreys,et al. Previewing distracters reduces their effective contrast , 2007, Vision Research.
[69] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[70] Prof. Dr. Valentino Braitenberg,et al. Anatomy of the Cortex , 1991, Studies of Brain Function.
[71] D. Prince,et al. GABAA receptor‐mediated currents in interneurons and pyramidal cells of rat visual cortex , 1998, The Journal of physiology.
[72] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. Spike-Frequency Adaptation of a Generalized Leaky Integrate-and-Fire Model Neuron , 2004, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.
[73] K. Lovblad,et al. Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[74] D. Heeger,et al. Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[75] Mieke Donk,et al. Prioritized selection in visual search through onset capture and color inhibition: evidence from a probe-dot detection task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[76] G. Humphreys,et al. Attention, spatial representation, and visual neglect: simulating emergent attention and spatial memory in the selective attention for identification model (SAIM). , 2003, Psychological review.
[77] S. Ferber,et al. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.
[78] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.
[79] Johan Hulleman,et al. Revisiting preview search at isoluminance: New onsets are not necessary for the preview advantage , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[80] Dietmar Heinke,et al. A computational model of visual marking using an inter-connected network of spiking neurons: The spiking search over time & space model (sSoTS) , 2006, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[81] Gustavo Deco,et al. Feature-based Attention in Human Visual Cortex: Simulation of Fmri Data , 2003 .
[82] Y. Tsal,et al. The attentional white bear phenomenon: the mandatory allocation of attention to expected distractor locations. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.