Limitations of feedforward control in multiple-phase steering movements

When attempting to perform bi-phasic steering movements (such as a lane change) in the absence of visual and inertial feedback, drivers produce a systematic heading error in the direction of the lane change (Wallis et al., Curr Biol 12(4):295–299, 2002; J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33(55):1127–1144, 2007). Theories of steering control which employ exclusively open-loop control mechanisms cannot accommodate this finding. In this article we show that a similar steering error occurs with obstacle avoidance, and offer compelling evidence that it stems from a seemingly general failure of human operators to correctly internalise the dynamics of the steering wheel. With respect to lateral position, the steering wheel is an acceleration control device, but we present data indicating that drivers treat it as a rate control device. Previous findings from Wallis et al. can be explained the same way. Since an open-loop control mechanism will never succeed when the dynamics of the controller are internalised improperly, we go on to conclude that regular, appropriately timed sensory feedback—predominantly from vision—is necessary for regulating heading, even during well-practiced, everyday manoeuvres such as lane changing and obstacle avoidance.

[1]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Go with the flow , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  J. Wann,et al.  Active gaze, visual look-ahead, and locomotor control. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  The role of visual and nonvisual information in the control of locomotion. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  P. Dixon,et al.  Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Constance S. Royden,et al.  From vision to action: experiments and models of steering control during driving. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Driving is smoother and more stable when using the tangent point. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[8]  Leslie Pack Kaelbling,et al.  A Dynamical Model of Visually-Guided Steering, Obstacle Avoidance, and Route Selection , 2003, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[9]  E C Poulton Tracking a variable rate of movement. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  Astros Chatziastros,et al.  The role of visual and nonvisual feedback in a vehicle steering task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  J. Thomson How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[13]  Poulton Ec,et al.  Tracking a variable rate of movement. , 1967 .

[14]  Christine Martin,et al.  Characteristics of voluntary visual sampling of the environment for safe locomotion over different terrains , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[16]  M A Goodale,et al.  The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  Guy Wallis,et al.  An Unexpected Role for Visual Feedback in Vehicle Steering Control , 2002, Current Biology.

[18]  John W. Senders,et al.  THE ATTENTIONAL DEMAND OF AUTOMOBILE DRIVING , 1967 .

[19]  Scott Glover,et al.  Visual illusions affect planning but not control , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[21]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Rob Gray,et al.  A Two-Point Visual Control Model of Steering , 2004, Perception.

[23]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Does gaze influence steering around a bend? , 2008, Journal of vision.

[24]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  David N. Lee,et al.  Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.

[26]  J. Wann,et al.  Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  J Godthelp Precognitive control: open- and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. , 1985, Ergonomics.

[28]  M. Bertamini,et al.  Understanding projectile acceleration. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  A E Patla,et al.  Where and when do we look as we approach and step over an obstacle in the travel path? , 1997, Neuroreport.

[30]  Steven Cloete,et al.  Changing lanes on a curved trajectory: Another look at visual feedback , 2008 .

[31]  J. Tresilian Hitting a moving target: Perception and action in the timing of rapid interceptions , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  A. Mizuno,et al.  A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..

[33]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[34]  Yuichiro Anzai,et al.  Cognitive Control of Real-Time Event-Driven Systems , 1984, Cogn. Sci..

[35]  S. Glover,et al.  Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  J. Thomson,et al.  The role of visual information in control of a constrained locomotor task. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  Michael Land,et al.  Which parts of the road guide steering? , 1995, Nature.

[38]  Ronald A. Hess,et al.  A Model of Driver Steering Control Behavior for Use in Assessing Vehicle Handling Qualities , 1993 .

[39]  Poulton Ec On the stimulus and response in pursuit tracking. , 1957 .

[40]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Driving as Night Falls The Contribution of Retinal Flow and Visual Direction to the Control of Steering , 2002, Current Biology.

[41]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Changing lanes: inertial cues and explicit path information facilitate steering performance when visual feedback is removed , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  John Wann,et al.  Heading in the wrong direction Reply to Fajen and Warren , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Richard Wilkie,et al.  Controlling steering and judging heading: retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  Brett R. Fajen,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2003 .

[45]  E C POULTON On the stimulus and response in pursuit tracking. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Naive physics: the curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.