Subjective and objective responses to ethanol in moderate/heavy and light social drinkers.

BACKGROUND Individuals who drink heavily are at an increased risk for adverse consequences of drinking and progression of their drinking habits to abuse or dependence. Therefore, it is important to delineate factors associated with their heavy drinking. METHODS We examined individual differences in subjective and objective responses to ethanol associated with level of consumption by reanalyzing data from the nine heaviest and nine lightest social drinkers from each of two independently collected subject samples: Holdstock and de Wit (1998) and King et al. (1997). The light drinkers in both samples consumed five or less alcoholic drinks per week, whereas the moderate/heavy drinkers consumed eight or more drinks per week with frequent binge episodes. Acute subjective and objective responses to ethanol (0.6 or 0.8 g/kg) or placebo were compared in the two groups at baseline and during rising and falling blood alcohol concentrations. RESULTS Moderate/heavy drinkers reported greater stimulant-like and fewer sedative-like and aversive subjective effects after ethanol than did lighter drinkers. These differences occurred in the absence of any group differences in breath alcohol levels, performance effects, or neuroendocrine changes or in overall reports of feeling any drug effects. CONCLUSIONS These data indicate that habitual moderate/heavy ethanol use was associated with greater stimulant-like effects after an acute dose of alcohol. This finding is consistent with the idea (Newlin and Thomson, 1990, 1999) that individuals who experience greater stimulant-like effects during the ascending limb and lesser sedative-like effects on the descending limb of the blood alcohol concentration curve may be at greater risk for developing ethanol use disorders. Although we cannot determine the causality of this association, sensitivity to the stimulant effects of ethanol may play an important role in the continuation of heavy ethanol use and the increased risk of negative consequences from this use.

[1]  M A Schuckit,et al.  Alcohol challenges in young men from alcoholic pedigrees and control families: a report from the COGA project. , 1996, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[2]  M. Earleywine,et al.  Development and validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. , 1993, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[3]  T. Smith,et al.  An 8-year follow-up of 450 sons of alcoholic and control subjects. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[4]  R. Pihl,et al.  The effects of alcohol on the cardiovascular stress response in men at high risk for alcoholism: a dose response study. , 1992, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[5]  D. Newlin,et al.  Alcohol challenge with sons of alcoholics: a critical review and analysis. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  M. Schuckit,et al.  Biological, psychological and environmental predictors of the alcoholism risk: a longitudinal study. , 1998, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[7]  H. de Wit,et al.  Relationship between subjective effects and drug preferences: ethanol and diazepam. , 1994, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[8]  R. Foltin,et al.  Utility of subjective-effects measurements in assessing abuse liability of drugs in humans. , 1991, British journal of addiction.

[9]  R. Radcliffe,et al.  Chronic ethanol consumption produces genotype-dependent tolerance to ethanol in LS/Ibg and SS/Ibg mice , 1992, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[10]  R. Wise,et al.  A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. , 1987, Psychological review.

[11]  S. P. Chou,et al.  Early onset of drinking as a risk factor for lifetime alcohol-related problems. , 1992, British journal of addiction.

[12]  R. Pihl,et al.  Cardiovascular reactivity as a predictor of alcohol consumption in a taste test situation. , 1994, Journal of clinical psychology.

[13]  T. Duka,et al.  Alcohol choice and outcome expectancies in social drinkers , 1998, Behavioural pharmacology.

[14]  H. de Wit,et al.  Ethanol impairs saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements without producing self-reports of sedation. , 1999, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[15]  A D Pokorny,et al.  The brief MAST: a shortened version of the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test. , 1972, The American journal of psychiatry.

[16]  M. Schuckit Alcohol sensitivity and dependence. , 1994, EXS.

[17]  M. Schuckit Low level of response to alcohol as a predictor of future alcoholism. , 1994, The American journal of psychiatry.

[18]  P. Conrod,et al.  Ethanol-induced change in cardiac and endogenous opiate function and risk for alcoholism. , 1996, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[19]  H. Kalant,et al.  Research on tolerance: what can we learn from history? , 1998, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[20]  M A Schuckit,et al.  Self-rating of alcohol intoxication by young men with and without family histories of alcoholism. , 1980, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[21]  H. de Wit,et al.  Testing the abuse liability of anxiolytic and hypnotic drugs in humans. , 1991, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[22]  C. Nagoshi,et al.  One-month repeatability of emotional responses to alcohol. , 1988, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[23]  P. Conrod,et al.  Biphasic effects of alcohol on heart rate are influenced by alcoholic family history and rate of alcohol ingestion. , 1997, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[24]  C. Gianoulakis,et al.  Enhanced sensitivity of pituitary beta-endorphin to ethanol in subjects at high risk of alcoholism. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  T. Phillips,et al.  Duration of sensitization to the locomotor stimulant effects of ethanol in mice , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[26]  H. Little Mechanisms that may underlie the behavioural effects of ethanol , 1991, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  D. Wechsler The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence, 4th ed. , 1958 .

[28]  R. Pihl,et al.  Men at high risk for alcoholism: the effect of alcohol on cardiovascular response to unavoidable shock. , 1987, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  H. de Wit,et al.  Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. , 1984, Psychological review.

[30]  D. Newlin,et al.  Chronic tolerance and sensitization to alcohol in sons of alcoholics: II. Replication and reanalysis. , 1999, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[31]  J. Volpicelli,et al.  Effect of naltrexone on subjective alcohol response in subjects at high and low risk for future alcohol dependence , 1997, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  H. de Wit,et al.  Individual differences in the biphasic effects of ethanol. , 1998, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[33]  D. Jasinski,et al.  Physiologic, subjective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine, methamphetamine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine, and methylphenidate in man , 1971, Clinical pharmacology and therapeutics.

[34]  M. Meaney,et al.  Different pituitary beta-endorphin and adrenal cortisol response to ethanol in individuals with high and low risk for future development of alcoholism. , 1989, Life sciences.

[35]  R. Pihl,et al.  Effects of alcohol on psychophysiological hyperreactivity to nonaversive and aversive stimuli in men at high risk for alcoholism. , 1990, Journal of abnormal psychology.