An Adaptive Response to Uncertainty Generates Positive and Negative Contrast Effects

Choice in Changing Environments Animals, including humans, generally tend to judge the world on relative, rather than absolute, terms. For example, the value of a particular object or reward is generally determined based on comparison to other rewards we have received in the past or to those that others have received. Such contrast effects can have negative or positive impacts on our behavior. McNamara et al. (p. 1084) used an optimality model to show that contrast effects could evolve as an adaptive response to environmental instability and unpredictability. Contrast effects, in which current behavior depends on past conditions, may be an adaptive response to uncertainty. Successive contrast effects, in which behavior is dependent on whether conditions are currently better or worse than they were before, are a striking illustration of the fact that animals evaluate the world in relative terms. Existing explanations for these effects are based on descriptive models of psychological and physiological processes, but little attention has been paid to the factors promoting their evolution. Using a simple and general optimality model, we show that contrast effects can result from an adaptive response to uncertainty in a changing, unpredictable world. A wide range of patterns of environmental change will select for sensitivity to past conditions, generating positive and negative contrast effects. Our analysis reveals the importance of incorporating uncertainty and environmental stochasticity into models of adaptive behavior.

[1]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[2]  Susan D. Healy,et al.  Context-dependent decisions among options varying in a single dimension , 2012, Behavioural Processes.

[3]  Christopher P. Puto,et al.  Adding Asymmetrically Dominated Alternatives: Violations of Regularity & the Similarity Hypothesis. , 1981 .

[4]  A. Houston,et al.  Integrating function and mechanism. , 2009, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[5]  Angel M. Elgier,et al.  Incentive contrast in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2009, Journal of Comparative Psychology.

[6]  R. Dantzer The Psychology of Fear and Stress, J.A. Gray (Ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1987), viii and 422 pp, ISBN 0-521-27098-7 , 1989 .

[7]  D. Zeaman,et al.  Response latency as a function of the amount of reinforcement. , 1949, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  M. Papini,et al.  Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in Mice , 2000 .

[9]  L. Lipsitt,et al.  A negative contrast effect in newborns. , 1972, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  A. Houston,et al.  The Common Currency for Behavioral Decisions , 1986, The American Naturalist.

[11]  A Houston,et al.  The application of statistical decision theory to animal behaviour. , 1980, Journal of theoretical biology.

[12]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[13]  P. Bednekoff Models of Adaptive Behaviour: An Approach based on State , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[15]  L. Crespi Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. , 1942 .

[16]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  Models of Adaptive Behaviour: An Approach Based on State , 1999 .

[17]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  Generalized Optimal Risk Allocation: Foraging and Antipredator Behavior in a Fluctuating Environment , 2012, The American Naturalist.

[18]  E. Capaldi,et al.  Repeated shifts in reward magnitude: evidence in favor of an associational and absolute (noncontextual) interpretation. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  A. Amsel Frustration Theory: An Analysis of Dispositional Learning and Memory , 1992 .

[20]  A. Houston,et al.  Environmental variability can select for optimism or pessimism. , 2011, Ecology letters.

[21]  Ola Olsson,et al.  Bayes' theorem and its applications in animal behaviour , 2006 .

[22]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[23]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Successive negative contrast in a bird: starlings' behaviour after unpredictable negative changes in food quality , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  Jan‐Åke Nilsson Metabolic consequences of hard work , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Successive negative contrast in the consummatory responding of didelphid marsupials , 1988 .

[26]  M E Bitterman,et al.  The overlearning-extinction effect and successive negative contrast in honeybees (Apis mellifera). , 1984, Journal of comparative psychology.

[27]  A. Houston,et al.  General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation , 1993 .

[28]  J. H. McHose,et al.  Partial reward, the negative contrast effect, and incentive averaging , 1975 .

[29]  J. F. Gilliam,et al.  Habitat Selection Under Predation Hazard: Test of a Model with Foraging Minnows. , 1987, Ecology.