Statistical regularities bias overt attention

[1]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional , 2018, Journal of cognition.

[2]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[3]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering , 2017, Cortex.

[4]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Michel F. Failing,et al.  People look at the object they fear: oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal threat , 2017, Cognition & emotion.

[6]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Selection history: How reward modulates selectivity of visual attention , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[7]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Mixed signals: The effect of conflicting reward- and goal-driven biases on selective attention , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[8]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Was That a Threat? Attentional Biases by Signals of Threat , 2017, Emotion.

[9]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Pavlovian reward learning underlies value driven attentional capture , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs , 2015, Psychological science.

[12]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal the availability of reward. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional capture by signals of threat , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[14]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Rewards teach visual selective attention , 2013, Vision Research.

[15]  N. Turk-Browne,et al.  Attention Is Spontaneously Biased Toward Regularities , 2013, Psychological science.

[16]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Reward grabs the eye: Oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli , 2012, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  E. Newport,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological Statistical Learning : from Acquiring Specific Items to Forming General Rules on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[19]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Reward on Attentional Learning: From Passive Associations to Active Monitoring , 2011, PloS one.

[20]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Evidence for a dissociation between the control of oculomotor capture and disengagement , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[22]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[23]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings II: Additivity is no evidence for filtering , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Early and late modulation of saccade deviations by target distractor similarity. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Should I stay or should I go? Attentional disengagement from visually unique and unexpected items at fixation. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  M. Roesch,et al.  Should I Stay or Should I Go? , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  Target location probability effects in visual search: an effect of sequential dependencies. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture with static singletons , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Probability Cuing of Target Location Facilitates Visual Search Implicitly in Normal Participants and Patients with Hemispatial Neglect , 2002, Psychological science.

[32]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  M. Chun,et al.  Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .

[34]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.

[36]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[39]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[40]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[41]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[47]  J Miller,et al.  Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[50]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[51]  M. Shaw,et al.  Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  J. D. Seifert,et al.  A THREAT. , 1964, Marquette medical review.

[53]  N. Clark,et al.  Direct Evidence , 1934 .