“Catastrophic” pathways to smoking cessation: findings from national survey

Abstract Objective To assess the extent to which the prevailing model of smoking cessation (that smokers typically prepare their attempts to stop smoking in advance and that doing so increases their chances of success) is correct. Design Cross sectional household survey. Setting England. Participants 918 smokers who reported having made at least one quit attempt and 996 ex-smokers aged 16 and over. Main outcome measures Whether the most recent quit attempt was planned in advance and whether quit attempts made at least six months before resulted in at least six months' abstinence. Results 48.6% of smokers reported that their most recent quit attempt was put into effect immediately the decision to quit was made. Unplanned quit attempts were more likely to succeed for at least six months: among respondents who had made a quit attempt between six months and five years previously the odds of success were 2.6 times higher (95% confidence interval 1.9 to 3.6) in unplanned attempts than in planned attempts; in quit attempts made 6-12 months previously the corresponding figure was 2.5 (1.4 to 4.7). The differences remained after controlling for age, sex, and socioeconomic group. Conclusions A model of the process of change based on “catastrophe theory” is proposed, in which smokers have varying levels of motivational “tension” to stop and then “triggers” in the environment result in a switch in motivational state. If that switch involves immediate renunciation of cigarettes, this can signal a more complete transformation than if it involves a plan to quit at some future point.

[1]  F. Huppert,et al.  Computerized cognitive examination of the elderly (ECO): The development of a neuropsychological examination for clinic and population use , 1993 .

[2]  Faruk Gul,et al.  A Theory of Addiction , 2001 .

[3]  J. Touchon,et al.  Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment , 2001, Neurology.

[4]  A. Remillard A pharmacoepidemiological evaluation of anticholinergic prescribing patterns in the elderly , 1996, Pharmacoepidemiology and drug safety.

[5]  R. West Time for a change: putting the Transtheoretical (Stages of Change) Model to rest. , 2005, Addiction.

[6]  Kenneth Rockwood,et al.  Variations in case definition affect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive impairment , 2003, Neurology.

[7]  C. Jack,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2004, Journal of internal medicine.

[8]  L C Larabie,et al.  To what extent do smokers plan quit attempts? , 2005, Tobacco Control.

[9]  K. Ritchie Mild cognitive impairment: an epidemiological perspective , 2004, Dialogues in clinical neuroscience.

[10]  Richard Doll,et al.  Mortality in relation to smoking: 22 years' observations on female British doctors. , 1980, British medical journal.

[11]  Graham K. Rand,et al.  Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences , 1983 .

[12]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Aging, memory, and the cholinergic system: A study of dichotic listening , 1980, Neurobiology of Aging.

[13]  Douglas Greve,et al.  Functional MRI detection of pharmacologically induced memory impairment , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  S. Ferris,et al.  Hypersensitivity to scopolamine in the elderly , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[15]  W. Velicer,et al.  The Transtheoretical Model of Health Behavior Change , 1997, American journal of health promotion : AJHP.

[16]  L. Hirsch Competing interests: none declared. , 2006 .

[17]  J. Touchon,et al.  The eugeria study of cognitive ageing: Who are the ‘normal’ elderly? , 1993 .

[18]  S. Ferris,et al.  Scopolamine effects on memory, language, visuospatial praxis and psychomotor speed , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[19]  J. Stockman,et al.  Mortality in Relation to Smoking: 50 Years' Observations on Male British Doctors , 2006 .