Defining Phonological Rules over Lexical Neighbourhoods: Evidence from Canadian Raising

Recent years have seen a heightened interest in incorporating a wide range of nonphonological factors, including phonetic, morphological, lexical, syntactic, social, and usage factors, into explaining phonological phenomena (see, for example, Beddor et al., 1986; Browman & Goldstein, 1989; Keating, 1990; Ohala, 1990, 1993; Flemming, 2001; Hume & Johnson, 2001; Pierrehumbert et al., 2001; Gordon, 2002; Beckman & Pierrehumbert, 2003; Bod et al., 2003; Bybee, 2003; Guy, 2003; Padgett, 2003; Hall et al., 2004, among others). This interest seems to stem from the idea that “phonology” involves the study of linguistic sound systems and whatever factors affect these systems, regardless of more traditional notions of modular grammar. One aspect of this trend has been an interest in lexically based accounts of grammar in general and phonology in particular (e.g., Lahiri & Marslen-Wilson, 1991; MacDonald et al., 1994; Goldinger, 1997; Bybee, 2000; Pallier et al., 2001; Beckman & Pierrehumbert, 2003; Hay et al., 2003). This paper provides further evidence for the idea that the understanding of phonological patterns can be usefully informed by looking at phonological neighbourhoods in the lexicon; that is, by considering word similarity. The focus of investigation is “Canadian raising,” a cover term for the distribution of the vowels [ai] and [i] in certain dialects of English, including many spoken in Canada. It will be shown that the traditional phonological accounts of Canadian raising as a categorical process, governed by the voicing of the following segment and stress patterns, are not in fact sufficient to account for the actual patterns of Canadian raising for at least some speakers. In addition to these factors, phonological neighbourhoods also play a role in shaping the distribution of [ai] and [i]. Section 2 gives a brief overview of Canadian raising and its role in the phonological literature, and outlines an elicitation study carried out to look at the modern pattern of Canadian raising in a particular Ontario community. Section 3 then describes the results of this study, and section 4 discusses how the results can be modelled making reference to phonological neighbourhoods. Finally, section 5 presents conclusions.

[1]  J. Chambers Canadian Raising: Blocking, Fronting, etc. , 1989 .

[2]  Brian D. Joseph,et al.  The handbook of historical linguistics , 2003 .

[3]  Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al.  The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution , 1994 .

[4]  Edward Flemming Scalar and categorical phenomena in a unified model of phonetics and phonology , 2001, Phonology.

[5]  John J. Ohala,et al.  There is no interface between phonology and phonetics: a personal view , 1990 .

[6]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Sympathy and phonological opacity , 1999, Phonology.

[7]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Paradigm Uniformity and the Phonetics-Phonology Boundary , 1996 .

[8]  Mary E. Beckman,et al.  Phonetic Interpretation Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Interpreting ‘phonetic interpretation’ over the lexicon , 2004 .

[9]  Mary E. Beckman,et al.  Phonetic Interpretation Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI: Speech perception, well-formedness and the statistics of the lexicon , 2004 .

[10]  Louis Goldstein,et al.  Articulatory gestures as phonological units , 1989, Phonology.

[11]  J. T. Jensen Against ambisyllabicity , 2000, Phonology.

[12]  John Kingston,et al.  Papers in Laboratory Phonology: Index of names , 1990 .

[13]  Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al.  Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast , 2000 .

[14]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The mental representation of lexical form: A phonological approach to the recognition lexicon , 1991, Cognition.

[15]  P. Boersma,et al.  Empirical Tests of the Gradual Learning Algorithm , 2001, Linguistic Inquiry.

[16]  Keith Johnson,et al.  The Role of Speech Perception in Phonology , 2001 .

[17]  Patricia A. Keating,et al.  Phonetic representations in a generative grammar , 1990 .

[18]  Louis Goldstein,et al.  Perceptual constraints and phonological change: a study of nasal vowel height , 1986, Phonology.

[19]  M. Gordon A Phonetically Driven Account of Syllable Weight , 2002 .

[20]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer , 2002 .

[21]  Sylvain Bromberger Why phonology is different , 1989 .

[22]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure , 2001 .

[23]  Carole Paradis,et al.  La règle de Canadian Raising et l’analyse en structure syllabique , 1980, Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique.

[24]  M. Joos A Phonological Dilemma in Canadian English , 1942 .

[25]  Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero,et al.  The acquisition of phonological opacity , 2003 .

[26]  C. Pallier,et al.  The Influence of Native-Language Phonology on Lexical Access: Exemplar-Based Versus Abstract Lexical Entries , 2001, Psychological science.

[27]  Joan Bybee Joan Bybee: Phonology and Language Use , 2004, Phonetica.