Some cognitive processes and their consequences for the organisation and presentation of information

Abstract It has been known for a considerable time that when dealing with complex intellectual tasks, the Limited processing capacity of working memory is critical. Nevertheless, many commonly used instructional techniques ignore this factor and consequently impose an excessive cognitive load that interferes with the major learning mechanisms of schema acquisition and automation. For example, when solving unfamiliar problems, strategies are frequently employed that are effective in arriving at a solution but because of the cognitive load imposed, are ineffective with respect to learning. Empirical evidence suggests that if learning is the goal, solving large numbers of conventional problems may not be appropriate. Instead, goal-free problems and worked examples can reduce extraneous cognitive load and facilitate schema acquisition and automation. Similarly, when presenting new material, information structures that require learners to unnecessarily split their attention between multiple sources of informat...

[1]  L M Reder,et al.  Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for the main points of a text , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[2]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[3]  J. Sweller,et al.  What do students learn while solving mathematics problems , 1985 .

[4]  Renae Low,et al.  Text editing of algebraic word problems , 1990 .

[5]  J. Bain,et al.  Information-Processing Demands of Transitive Inference , 1986 .

[6]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[7]  J. Bain,et al.  Capacity Limitations in Children's Reasoning: A Dual-Task Approach. , 1986 .

[8]  John R. Anderson,et al.  A Comparison of Texts and their Summaries: Memorial Consequences. , 1980 .

[9]  Robin Jeffries,et al.  The Processes Involved in Designing Software. , 1980 .

[10]  J. Sweller,et al.  Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. , 1983 .

[11]  A. D. D. Groot Thought and Choice in Chess , 1978 .

[12]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[13]  R. A. Tarmizi,et al.  Guidance during Mathematical Problem Solving. , 1988 .

[14]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[15]  H. Simon,et al.  Perception in chess , 1973 .

[16]  J. Sweller,et al.  Locus of Difficulty in Multistage Mathematics Problems , 1990 .

[17]  J. Sweller,et al.  Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. , 1987 .

[18]  J. Sweller,et al.  The Use of Worked Examples as a Substitute for Problem Solving in Learning Algebra , 1985 .

[19]  Renae Low,et al.  Hierarchical Ordering of Schematic Knowledge Relating to Area-of-Rectangle Problems. , 1992 .

[20]  H. Simon,et al.  A simulation of memory for chess positions. , 1973 .

[21]  J. Sweller,et al.  Structuring Effective Worked Examples , 1990 .

[22]  J. Schooler,et al.  Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  M. Levine,et al.  Effects of Goal Specificity on Means-Ends Analysis and Learning. , 1982 .

[24]  D E Egan,et al.  Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings , 1979, Memory & cognition.

[25]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive technology: Some procedures for facilitating learning and problem solving in mathematics and science. , 1989 .

[26]  H. Simon,et al.  Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  R T Solman,et al.  The effect of pictures on the acquisition of a small vocabulary of similar sight-words. , 1984, The British journal of educational psychology.

[28]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Models of Competence in Solving Physics Problems , 1980, Cogn. Sci..

[29]  Paul Ayres Why Goal-Free Problems Can Facilitate Learning , 1993 .

[30]  R. Solman,et al.  Effective use of pictures as extra stimulus prompts. , 1993, The British journal of educational psychology.

[31]  P. Chandler,et al.  THE SPLIT‐ATTENTION EFFECT AS A FACTOR IN THE DESIGN OF INSTRUCTION , 1992 .

[32]  H. Simon,et al.  Learning Mathematics From Examples and by Doing , 1987 .

[33]  P. Chandler,et al.  Evidence for Cognitive Load Theory , 1991 .

[34]  E. James Kehoe,et al.  Pictures Block the Learning of Sightwords , 1992 .

[35]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Abstract Planning and Perceptual Chunks: Elements of Expertise in Geometry , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[36]  H A Simon,et al.  How Big Is a Chunk? , 1974, Science.

[37]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical material. , 1990 .

[39]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .