A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning.

A selective review of experiments that can be said to demonstrate the effects of generalization decrement in Pavlovian condition is presented, and it is argued that an adequate theoretical explanation for them is currently not available. This article then develops a theoretical account for the processes of generalization and generalization decrement in Pavlovian conditioning. It assumes that animals represent their environment by a stimulus array in a buffer and that this array in its entirety constitutes the conditioned stimulus. Generalization is then held to occur whenever at least some of the stimuli represented in the array on a test trial are the same as at least some of those represented in the array during training. Specifically, the magnitude of generalization is determined by the proportion of the array occupied by these common stimuli during training compared to the proportion of the array they occupy during testing. By adding to this principle rules concerning excitatory and inhibitory learning, it is proposed, the model can explain all the results that were difficult for its predecessors to account for. A fundamental property of conditioned responding is that it may occur in circumstances that differ from those prevailing during acquisition. This phenomenon is referred to as generalization, and the term generalization decrement is used when this transfer is less than complete. These effects have been revealed with both Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning, using a variety of species and a wide range of conditioned stimuli (CS) and unconditioned stimuli (US). As iar as instrumental conditioning is concerned, a considerable number of studies have been directed at examining the factors that influence the magnitude of generalization from one situation to another (for reviews see Honig & Urcuioli, 1981; Mackintosh, 1974). These results also have been the focus of considerable theoretical analysis. In contrast, much less attention has been paid to the role of generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. Only a few studies have been concerned specifically with demonstrating the generalization decrement of a Pavlovian conditioned response (CR) as a result of modifying the properties of the CS (Hofiman & Fleshier, 1961; Hovland, 1937; Moore, 1972; Pavlov, 1927; Siegel, Hearst, George, & O'Neal, 1968). Furthermore, only a few theorists have been specifically concerned with the analysis of these and related effects (Hull, 1943; Pavlov, 1927). An alternative approach has been to show how a particular theoretical analysis of the Pavlovian learning process can be extended to account for generalization and generalization decrement (cf. Rescorla, 1976). The argument presented in this article is based on the premise that an adequate account for the effects of generalization must lie at the core of

[1]  C. I. Hovland The Generalization of Conditioned Responses: I. The Sensory Generalization of Conditioned Responses with Varying Frequencies of Tone , 1937 .

[2]  Harold Gulliksen,et al.  A theory of learning and transfer: I , 1938 .

[3]  C. B. Woodbury The learning of stimulus patterns by dogs. , 1943 .

[4]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[5]  C. L. Hull,et al.  A Behavior System , 1954 .

[6]  R. Lubow,et al.  Latent inhibition: the effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. , 1959, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[7]  H. Hoffman,et al.  Stimulus factors in aversive controls: the generalization of conditioned suppression. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[8]  Eugene Galanter,et al.  Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .

[9]  Morton P. Friedman,et al.  Transfer effects in discrimination learning , 1964 .

[10]  H. Hoffman,et al.  STIMULUS ASPECTS OF AVERSIVE CONTROLS: THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE CONTINGENT SHOCK. , 1965, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Perception and the Conditioned Reflex , 1965 .

[12]  Richard C. Atkinson,et al.  Stimulus Sampling Theory , 1967 .

[13]  E. Fischer Conditioned Reflexes , 1942, American journal of physical medicine.

[14]  J. Konorski Integrative activity of the brain , 1967 .

[15]  K. Haberlandt,et al.  Stimulus selection in animal discrimination learning. , 1968, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[16]  Generalization gradients obtained from individual subjects following classical conditioning. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  R. Rescorla Pavlovian conditioned inhibition , 1969 .

[18]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Fundamental issues in associative learning : proceedings of a symposium held at Dalhousie University, Halifax, June 1968 , 1969 .

[19]  R. Rescorla Reduction in the effectiveness of reinforcement after prior excitatory conditioning , 1970 .

[20]  N. J. Mackintosh,et al.  An Analysis of Overshadowing and Blocking , 1971 .

[21]  K. Spence,et al.  Essays in neobehaviorism : a memorial volume to Kenneth W. Spence , 1971 .

[22]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[23]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[24]  P. W. Frey,et al.  Inhibition and learning , 1973 .

[25]  N. Mackintosh The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .

[26]  D. Blough Steady state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination. , 1975 .

[27]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[28]  D. Medin A theory of context in discrimination learning , 1975 .

[29]  R. Rescorla Stimulus generalization: some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[30]  V. Lolordo,et al.  Control by the auditory or the visual element of a compound discriminative stimulus: effects of feedback. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[31]  Configural-like effects observed in the course of second-order conditioning in rats☆ , 1977 .

[32]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Stimulus similarity as a determinant of Pavlovian conditioning. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[33]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Loss of control by a less valid or by a less salient stimulus compounded with a better predictor of reinforcement , 1977 .

[34]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Handbook of Operant Behavior , 2022 .

[35]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Within-compound flavor associations. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[36]  P. W. Frey,et al.  Model of conditioning incorporating the Rescorla-Wagner associative axiom, a dynamic attention process, and a catastrophe rule. , 1978 .

[37]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[38]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  One-Trial Overshadowing , 1979 .

[39]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.

[40]  R. Rescorla Simultaneous and successive associations in sensory preconditioning. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[41]  A. R. Wagner,et al.  One-trial overshadowing: evidence of distributive processing. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[42]  JOHN W. Moore,et al.  Erratum to: Formation of attentional-associative networks in real time: Role of the hippocampus and implications for conditioning , 1980 .

[43]  W. K. Honig,et al.  The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): Twenty-five years of research on stimulus generalization. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[44]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Blocking with a single compound trial , 1982 .

[45]  R. Rescorla Some consequences of associations between the excitor and the inhibitor in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. , 1982 .

[46]  S. J. Weiss,et al.  The influence of positive and negative reinforcement on selective attention in the rat , 1982 .

[47]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .

[48]  J. Pearce,et al.  The Influence of Generalization Decrement on the Outcome of a Feature-Positive Discrimination , 1984 .

[49]  P. Balsam,et al.  Context and Learning , 1985 .

[50]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Information processing in animals : conditioned inhibition , 1985 .

[51]  E. Kehoe,et al.  Summation and configuration in patterning schedules with the rat and rabbit , 1985 .

[52]  Cue-context interactions in discrimination, categorization, and memory , 1985 .