Grey Scales uncover similar attentional effects in homonymous hemianopia and visual hemi-neglect

Grey Scales uncover similar attentional effects in homonymous hemianopia and visual hemi-neglect * 1. Abstract Multi-component models of visual hemi-neglect have postulated that visual hemi-neglect is characterised by various attentional deficits. A Grey Scales-task has been developed to quantify the early, automatic, (perhaps obligatory) ipsilesional orienting of visual attention, frequently assumed as the first of these attentional deficits. Explanations for this attentional imbalance are up until now mainly formulated in terms of right hemisphere activation. This lateral attentional bias has also been demonstrated in controls, in whom it is expressed as a leftward perceptual asymmetry. We reproduced previous literature findings on a Grey Scales-task, considering controls and neglect patients. Three patients with neglect showed an extreme ipsilesional lateral bias. This bias did not change during or after cognitive rehabilitation. Additionally, we presented this Grey Scale-task to 32 patients with left-and right-sided homonymous hemianopia (HH). Homonymous hemianopia is the loss of sight in one visual hemi-field. The HH patients had no clinical signs of impaired lateralised attention. Results revealed that HH patients showed a similar ipsilesional bias, albeit to a lesser degree than in neglect. Left-sided HH patients presented a quantitatively similar, but qualitatively opposite bias than the right-sided HH patients. We suggest that sensory effects can be an alternative source of attentional imbalance, which can interact with the previously proposed (right) hemispheric effects. This suggests that the perceptual asymmetry in the Grey Scales-task is not necessarily an indicator of impaired right hemisphere attention. It rather suggests a pattern of functional cerebral asymmetry, which can also be caused by asymmetric sensory input. suggested that the clinical syndrome of unilateral visual spatial neglect (UN) can be described/explained as a series of successive attentional events beginning with (1) an early, automatic, chronic, perhaps obligatory, orienting of attention toward the ipsilesional half space, followed by (2) a deficit in disengaging attention from that side in order to reorient it toward the contralateral half space. In addition to these two deficits, (3) a generalised (i.e. directionally non-specific) reduction in attentional-information processing capacity is assumed. The first component underlies an anomalous lateral preference. The second component gives rise to the clinical signs of UN (e.g. left-sided omissions on cancellation tasks) [23]. Karnath [16] proposed that this second component (reorienting) recovers faster than the other two, and this has been confirmed by several authors (e.g. [28, 23]). Mattingley and colleagues [23] concluded that the apparent recovery of …

[1]  M. Albert A simple test of visual neglect , 1973, Neurology.

[2]  E. Renzi,et al.  Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  J. Zihl,et al.  Visual scanning behavior in patients with homonymous hemianopia , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  L. Gauthier,et al.  Evaluation of left visuospatial neglect: Norms and discrimination power of two tests. , 1990 .

[5]  Ulrich Oechsner,et al.  Evidence for scanpaths in hemianopic patients shown through string editing methods , 1996 .

[6]  K. Schaie,et al.  Age difference patterns of psychometric intelligence in adulthood: generalizability within and across ability domains. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[7]  Hiroshi Tsukagoshi,et al.  Eye-fixation patterns in homonymous hemianopia and unilateral spatial neglect , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  A C Kooijman,et al.  Does age change the distribution of visual attention? A comment on McCalley, Bouwhuis, and Juola (1995). , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[9]  L. Stark,et al.  Saccadic eye movement strategies in patients with homonymous hemianopia , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[10]  M. Brigell,et al.  Effects of Age and Hemianopic Visual Field Loss on Driving , 1993, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[11]  D. Elliott,et al.  Confrontation visual field tests. , 1997, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[12]  W. H. Zangemeister,et al.  Eye-head coordination in homonymous hemianopia , 2004, Journal of Neurology.

[13]  K Ball,et al.  Identifying Correlates of Accident Involvement for the Older Driver , 1991, Human factors.

[14]  J. Zihl Oculomotor scanning performance in subjects with homonymous visual field disorders , 1999 .

[15]  J. Zihl Rehabilitation of visual impairments in patients with brain damage , 1994 .

[16]  F. Cornelissen,et al.  Grey scales uncover similar attentional effects in homonymous hemianopia and visual hemi-neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  L. Pizzamiglio,et al.  Cognitive rehabilitation of the hemineglect disorder in chronic patients with unilateral right brain damage. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[18]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[19]  G. Kerkhoff Restorative and compensatory therapy approaches in cerebral blindness - a review. , 1999, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[20]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Visual hemispatial inattention: stimulus parameters and exploratory strategies. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[21]  S. Paolucci,et al.  Effectiveness of neglect rehabilitation in a randomized group study. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.