Infants' speech and gesture production in Mozambique and the Netherlands

In this paper, we explore the cultural differences in the production of speech and speech+gesture combinations by infants at the age of 17-18 months in Mozambique and the Netherlands. We found that Dutch infants produce more speech and gestures compared to Mozambican infants. Infants in both communities make most use of content words. The results further show that Dutch infants make more use of proximal pointing than Mozambicans, whereas Mozambicans make more use of the offering gesture. Finally, the amount of semantically coherent speech+gesture combinations of the Mozambican infants is higher than of the Dutch infants.

[1]  P. Vogt,et al.  Communicative intentions of child-directed speech in three different learning environments: Observations from the Netherlands, and rural and urban Mozambique , 2015 .

[2]  P. Vogt,et al.  Infant engagement and early vocabulary development: a naturalistic observation study of Mozambican infants from 1;1 to 2;1* , 2015, Journal of Child Language.

[3]  R. Levine,et al.  Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa. , 1996 .

[4]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. , 2011, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[5]  Ulf Liszkowski,et al.  Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. , 2013, Child development.

[6]  Brian Richards,et al.  Input and interaction in language acquisition: Introduction , 1994 .

[7]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Learning words by hand: Gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development , 2008, First language.

[8]  E. Lieven Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children , 1994 .

[9]  Claire Hughes,et al.  Early Social Cognition , 2010 .

[10]  Rebecca J. Panagos Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children , 1998 .

[11]  Paul Vogt,et al.  Rural and urban differences in language socialization and early vocabulary development in Mozambique , 2013, CogSci.

[12]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  How gesture promotes learning throughout childhood. , 2009, Child development perspectives.

[13]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Language and Gesture: Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: when hand and mouth come together , 2000 .

[14]  Hirokazu Yoshikawa,et al.  Ethnic differences in mother-infant language and gestural communications are associated with specific skills in infants. , 2012, Developmental science.

[15]  Patricia Zukow-Goldring,et al.  Sensitive Caregiving Fosters the Comprehension of Speech: When Gestures Speak Louder than Words , 1996 .

[16]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Gesture Paves the Way for Language Development , 2005, Psychological science.

[17]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Pointing Toward Two-Word Speech in Young Children , 2003 .

[18]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. , 2009, Developmental science.

[19]  Heidi Keller,et al.  Autonomy and Relatedness Revisited: Cultural Manifestations of Universal Human Needs , 2012 .

[20]  E. Hoff How social contexts support and shape language development , 2006 .

[21]  Madhavilatha Maganti,et al.  Cross-cultural evidence for multimodal motherese: Asian Indian mothers' adaptive use of synchronous words and gestures. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: Early communication in Italian vs. American children , 2008, First language.

[23]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Differences in Early Gesture Explain SES Disparities in Child Vocabulary Size at School Entry , 2009, Science.