No Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning During Exposure to Artificial Languages in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

BACKGROUND Language delay is a hallmark feature of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The identification of word boundaries in continuous speech is a critical first step in language acquisition that can be accomplished via statistical learning and reliance on speech cues. Importantly, early word segmentation skills have been shown to predict later language development in typically developing (TD) children. METHODS Here we investigated the neural correlates of online word segmentation in children with and without ASD with a well-established behavioral paradigm previously validated for functional magnetic resonance imaging. Eighteen high-functioning boys with ASD and 18 age- and IQ-matched TD boys underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging while listening to two artificial languages (containing statistical or statistical + prosodic cues to word boundaries) and a random speech stream. RESULTS Consistent with prior findings, in TD control subjects, activity in fronto-temporal-parietal networks decreased as the number of cues to word boundaries increased. The ASD children, however, did not show this facilitatory effect. Furthermore, statistical contrasts modeling changes in activity over time identified significant learning-related signal increases for both artificial languages in basal ganglia and left temporo-parietal cortex only in TD children. Finally, the level of communicative impairment in ASD children was inversely correlated with signal increases in these same regions during exposure to the artificial languages. CONCLUSIONS This is the first study to demonstrate significant abnormalities in the neural architecture subserving language-related learning in ASD children and to link the communicative impairments observed in this population to decreased sensitivity to the statistical and speech cues available in the language input.

[1]  W. McMahon,et al.  Superior Temporal Gyrus, Language Function, and Autism , 2007, Developmental neuropsychology.

[2]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[3]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Reading the Mind in the Voice: A Study with Normal Adults and Adults with Asperger Syndrome and High Functioning Autism , 2002, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  R. Gómez,et al.  Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[5]  Roger P. Woods,et al.  Characterizing volume and surface deformations in an atlas framework: theory, applications, and implementation , 2003, NeuroImage.

[6]  Patricia Howlin,et al.  Outcome in High-Functioning Adults with Autism with and Without Early Language Delays: Implications for the Differentiation Between Autism and Asperger Syndrome , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[7]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  Can Infants Map Meaning to Newly Segmented Words? , 2007, Psychological science.

[8]  Skye McDonald,et al.  An Exploration of Causes of Non-Literal Language Problems in Individuals with Asperger Syndrome , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  J. Buitelaar,et al.  The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: An integrative review , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[10]  D. Geschwind,et al.  Genome-wide analyses of human perisylvian cerebral cortical patterning , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  P. Hagoort,et al.  Neural correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Michael S. Gaffrey,et al.  Atypical [corrected] participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: an fMRI study of semantic decision. , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  When cues collide: use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[14]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  The neural basis of speech parsing in children and adults. , 2010, Developmental science.

[15]  G. Bartolucci,et al.  Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children , 1980, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[16]  A. Bailey,et al.  Autism: towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[18]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Dissociating Neural Mechanisms of Temporal Sequencing and Processing Phonemes , 2003, Neuron.

[19]  Michael S. Gaffrey,et al.  A typical participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: An fMRI study of semantic decision , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  R. Gómez,et al.  Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  M. Reite,et al.  Smaller left hemisphere planum temporale in adults with autistic disorder , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[22]  V S Caviness,et al.  Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: a nested whole-brain analysis. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  C. Chabris,et al.  Brain activation during semantic processing in autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  D. Geschwind,et al.  Evidence for a language quantitative trait locus on chromosome 7q in multiplex autism families. , 2002, American journal of human genetics.

[25]  J. Sebat,et al.  Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. , 2008, American journal of human genetics.

[26]  B. Leventhal,et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[27]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[28]  M. Just,et al.  Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  David A. Ziegler,et al.  Language‐association cortex asymmetry in autism and specific language impairment , 2004, Annals of neurology.

[30]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  At 11 months, prosody still outranks statistics. , 2009, Developmental science.

[31]  David A. Ziegler,et al.  Abnormal asymmetry in language association cortex in autism , 2002, Annals of neurology.

[32]  C D Frith,et al.  On the benefits of not trying: brain activity and connectivity reflecting the interactions of explicit and implicit sequence learning. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .

[34]  M. Dapretto,et al.  Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: the role of prosody and context. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  Language network specializations: An analysis with parallel task designs and functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  Ann Chadwick-Dias,et al.  A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and down syndrome children , 1990, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[37]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Early Word Segmentation by Infants and Toddlers With Williams Syndrome , 2003 .

[38]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses , 1999, NeuroImage.

[39]  Elizabeth Redcay,et al.  Deviant Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Patterns of Brain Activity to Speech in 2–3-Year-Old Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[40]  F. Happé Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory , 1993, Cognition.

[41]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: a retrospective analysis. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[42]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Segmenting nonsense: an event-related potential index of perceived onsets in continuous speech , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Just,et al.  Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children , 2020, Definitions.

[45]  M. Dapretto,et al.  Reading affect in the face and voice: neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[46]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups , 2001, Language and cognitive processes.

[47]  G. Bartolucci,et al.  Deictic categories in the language of autistic children , 1974, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[48]  C. Lord,et al.  A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[49]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cracking the Language Code: Neural Mechanisms Underlying Speech Parsing , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.