The Emergence of Collaborative Brain Function: fMRI Studies of the Development of Response Inhibition

Abstract: Adolescence marks the beginning of adult‐level cognitive control of behavior supported by the brain maturation processes of synaptic pruning and myelination. Cognitive development studies on adolescence indicate that this period is characterized by improvements in the performance of existing abilities including speed and capacity of information processing and the ability to have consistent cognitive control of behavior. Although adolescents can behave at adult levels in some ways, brain imaging studies indicate that the functional organization of brain systems that support higher‐cognitive processes are not fully mature. Synaptic pruning allows for more efficient local computations, enhancing the ability of discrete brain regions to support high‐level cognitive control of behavior including working memory. Myelination increases the speed of neuronal transmission supporting the collaboration of a widely distributed circuitry, integrating regions that support top‐down cognitive control of behavior. Functional brain imaging methods allow for the characterization of the relationship between cognitive development and brain maturation as we can map progressions in the establishment of distributed brain circuitry and its relation to enhanced cognitive control of behavior. We present a review on the maturation (as distinct from “development” in emphasizing the transition to maturity and stabilization) of response inhibition, brain structure, and brain function through adolescence. We also propose a model for brain‐behavior maturation that allows for the qualitative changes in cognitive processes that occur during adolescence.

[1]  P. Yakovlev,et al.  The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .

[2]  L. Wise,et al.  Decrement in Stroop Interference Time with Age , 1975 .

[3]  J. Changeux,et al.  Selective stabilisation of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks , 1976, Nature.

[4]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[5]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. , 1986, Science.

[6]  P. Goldman-Rakic Development of cortical circuitry and cognitive function. , 1987, Child Development.

[7]  R. Thatcher,et al.  Human cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and ages. , 1987, Science.

[8]  P. Goldman-Rakic Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  S. Tipper,et al.  Mechanisms of attention: a developmental study. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[10]  Karl H. Pribram,et al.  Stages of Brain and Cognitive Maturation , 1990 .

[11]  P. Huttenlocher Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  T. Paus,et al.  Development of an ability to maintain verbally instructed central gaze fixation studied in 8- to 10-year-old children. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  L Hasher,et al.  Age and inhibition. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  T. Jernigan,et al.  Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning , 1991 .

[16]  M. Dennis Frontal lobe function in childhood and adolescence: A heuristic for assessing attention regulation, executive control, and the intentional states important for social discourse , 1991 .

[17]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging , 1992 .

[18]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Biological and psychological development of executive functions , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  L M Greenberg,et al.  Developmental normative data on the test of variables of attention (T.O.V.A.). , 1993, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[20]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task , 1993, Nature.

[21]  D. Mathalon,et al.  A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[22]  P. Strick,et al.  Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus during cognitive processing. , 1994, Science.

[23]  D. Levy,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  D. Bjorklund,et al.  The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior , 1995 .

[25]  A. Schleicher,et al.  The ontogeny of human gyrification. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: Ages 4–18 years , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[27]  A. Diamond,et al.  Development of an aspect of executive control: development of the abilities to remember what I said and to "do as I say, not as I do". , 1996, Developmental psychobiology.

[28]  M. Mintun,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G Buzsáki,et al.  The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  A. Reiss,et al.  Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Antisaccade performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field , 1997, Nature.

[32]  C. Bruce,et al.  Suppression of task-related saccades by electrical stimulation in the primate's frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Valentino Bettinardi,et al.  Neural control of fast-regular saccades and antisaccades: an investigation using positron emission tomography , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  R Freedman,et al.  Age Diminishes Performance on an Antisaccade Eye Movement Task , 1997, Neurobiology of Aging.

[36]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation , 1997, Brain Research.

[37]  C. Nelson,et al.  The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  H. Chugani A critical period of brain development: studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. , 1998, Preventive medicine.

[39]  David H. Zald,et al.  The development of spatial working memory abilities , 1998 .

[40]  D. Munoz,et al.  Age-related performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  R M Müri,et al.  Functional organisation of saccades and antisaccades in the frontal lobe in humans: a study with echo planar functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[42]  H. Petsche,et al.  Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  P. Davies,et al.  Assessment of cognitive development in adolescents by means of neuropsychological tasks , 1999 .

[44]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Logan,et al.  Development of inhibitory control across the life span. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[46]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. , 1999, Science.

[47]  J. Sweeney,et al.  Cognitive functional magnetic resonance imaging at very-high-field: eye movement control. , 1999, Topics in magnetic resonance imaging : TMRI.

[48]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  H. Lee Swanson,et al.  What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[50]  F. Varela,et al.  Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity , 1999, Nature.

[51]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  A study of adaptive behavior: effects of age and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one's actions , 1999 .

[52]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Activity of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey during an antisaccade task , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  K. Fukushima,et al.  Development of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements I. Age-related changes in normal children , 2000, Brain and Development.

[54]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Functional frontalisation with age: mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[55]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  N. Minshew,et al.  Maturation of Widely Distributed Brain Function Subserves Cognitive Development , 2001, NeuroImage.

[57]  R. Berman,et al.  Inhibitory control of attention declines more than working memory during normal aging , 2001, Neurobiology of Aging.

[58]  C. Klein,et al.  Development of prosaccade and antisaccade task performance in participants aged 6 to 26 years. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[59]  A. Toga,et al.  Mapping Continued Brain Growth and Gray Matter Density Reduction in Dorsal Frontal Cortex: Inverse Relationships during Postadolescent Brain Maturation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[61]  Vicki A. Anderson,et al.  Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[62]  Gary H. Glover,et al.  A Developmental fMRI Study of the Stroop Color-Word Task , 2002, NeuroImage.

[63]  N. Minshew,et al.  Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory , 2002, Neurology.

[64]  V. Menon,et al.  Maturation of brain function associated with response inhibition. , 2002, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[65]  L. Wilkins July 9 Highlights , 2002 .

[66]  A. Demetriou,et al.  The development of mental processing: efficiency, working memory, and thinking. , 2002, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[67]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.

[68]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.