CHAPTER 13 “ Information ” and the chickadee call : Communicating with a complex vocal system
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Wilson,et al. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis , 1975 .
[2] Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al. Chick-a-dee call syntax, social context, and season affect vocal responses of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[3] K. Wells. Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach , 1999 .
[4] M. Ficken,et al. A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF PASSERINE MOBBING CALLS , 1996 .
[5] Jack P. Hailman,et al. The chick-a-dee call system of the Mexican chickadee , 1994 .
[6] Svein Haftorn. Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the Willow Tit Parus montanus , 1993 .
[7] D. Leger. Contextual sources of information and responses to animal communication signals. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[8] S. Nowicki,et al. Flock-specific recognition of chickadee calls , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[9] Susan T. Smith. Communication and other social behavior in Parus carolinensis , 1972 .
[10] Jack P. Hailman,et al. Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘Language’ by structural linguistics , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[11] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[12] M. Ficken. Call Similarities among Mixed Species Flock Associates , 2000 .
[13] Robert W. Ficken,et al. VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEE , 1978 .
[14] M. C. Baker,et al. Black-capped chickadee call dialects along a continuous habitat corridor , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[15] Philip K Gaddis,et al. STRUCTURE AND VARIABILITY IN THE VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE MOUNTAIN CHICKADEE , 1985 .
[16] Todd M Freeberg,et al. Receivers respond differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note composition in Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[17] D. Blumstein,et al. Does Sociality Drive The Evolution Of Communicative Complexity? A Comparative Test With Ground‐Dwelling Sciurid Alarm Calls , 1997, The American Naturalist.
[18] C. E. Ho,et al. A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[19] J. Hailman. Optical Signals: Animal Communication and Light , 1977 .
[20] Jack P. Hailman,et al. Constraints on the Structure of Combinatorial “Chick-a-dee” Calls , 1987 .
[21] F. Sheldon,et al. PHYLOGENY OF TITMICE (PARIDAE): II. SPECIES RELATIONSHIPS BASED ON SEQUENCES OF THE MITOCHONDRIAL CYTOCHROME-B GENE , 2005 .
[22] Todd M Freeberg,et al. Variation in chick-a-dee calls of a Carolina chickadee population, Poecile carolinensis: identity and redundancy within note types. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[23] Jack W. Bradbury,et al. Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .
[24] Thomas R. Nilsen. On defining communication , 1957 .
[25] April M. Becker,et al. MOBBING CALLS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES: EFFECTS OF URGENCY ON CALL PRODUCTION , 2002 .
[26] J. P. Hailman,et al. The organization of major vocalizations in the paridae , 1989 .
[27] R. W. Ficken,et al. Dialects in a call associated with pair interactions in the black-capped chickadee , 1985 .
[28] Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al. Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: The chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) , 2004 .
[29] Peter Marler,et al. Bird calls: just emotional displays or something more? , 2008 .
[30] William J. Smith,et al. The Behavior of Communicating: An Ethological Approach , 1980 .
[31] S. Nowicki,et al. Vocal plasticity in captive black-capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call convergence , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[32] Christine R. Hurd. Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[33] M. C. Baker,et al. Population Differentiation in a Complex Bird Sound: A Comparison of Three Bioacoustical Analysis Procedures , 2003 .
[34] Dorothy L. Mammen,et al. Individual differences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[35] J. Ekman. ECOLOGY OF NON-BREEDING SOCIAL SYSTEMS OF PARUS , 1989 .
[36] Susan M. Smith. The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral Ecology and Natural History , 1992 .
[37] S. Haftorn. CONTEXTS AND POSSIBLE FUNCTIONS OF ALARM CALLING IN THE WILLOW TIT, PARUS MONTANUS; THE PRINCIPLE OF ‘BETTER SAFE THAN SORRY’ , 2000 .
[38] R. Weisman,et al. Open-ended categorization of chick-a-dee calls by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[39] G. Wilkinson. 12. Social and Vocal Complexity in Bats , 2003 .
[40] Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al. Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. II: The chick-a-dee call of the mountain chickadee (Poecile gambeli) , 2004 .
[41] J. Hailman,et al. The ‘chick-a-dee’ calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English , 1985 .
[42] J. Lucas,et al. Carolina Chickadee (Aves, Paridae, Poecile carolinensis) Vocalization Rates: Effects of Body Mass and Food Availability under Aviary Conditions , 1999 .
[43] R. Dawkins,et al. Animal signals: information or manipulation? , 1978 .
[44] R G Weisman,et al. Call-note discriminations in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.
[45] DAVID MILLER,et al. Information and Communication , 1966, Nature.