Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial?

Three experiments investigated whether the presence of more elements in the upper part of a configuration (i.e., up-down asymmetry) plays a role in determining newborns' preference for facelike patterns. Newborns preferred a nonfacelike stimulus with more elements in the upper part over a nonfacelike stimulus with more elements in the lower part (Experiment 1), did not show a preference for a facelike stimulus over a nonfacelike configuration equated for the number of elements in the upper part of the configuration (Experiment 2), and preferred a nonfacelike configuration located in the upper portion of the stimulus over a facelike configuration in the lower portion of the pattern (Experiment 3). Results demonstrated that up-down asymmetry is crucial in determining newborns' face preference.

[1]  Models of neonates' preferences for facelike patterns: A response to Morton, Johnson, and Maurer , 1990 .

[2]  C. Umilta,et al.  Newborns’ preference for up–down asymmetrical configurations , 2002 .

[3]  K. A. Kleiner Specific VS. Non-Specific Face Recognition Device , 1993 .

[4]  M S Banks,et al.  Infant pattern vision: a new approach based on the contrast sensitivity function. , 1981, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  D. Maurer,et al.  Newborn's following of natural and distorted arrangements of facial features , 1983 .

[6]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. , 1991, Psychological review.

[7]  P. Green Biology and Cognitive Development: the Case of Face Recognition, Mark H. Johnson, John Morton. Blackwell, Oxford (1991), x, +180. Price £35.00 hardback, £10.95 paperback , 1992 .

[8]  J Wattam-Bell,et al.  Changes in Infants' Ability to Switch Visual Attention in the First Three Months of Life , 1992, Perception.

[9]  Sylvia M. J. Hains,et al.  Faceness or complexity: Evidence from newborn visual tracking of facelike stimuli , 1999 .

[10]  G. J. van der Wildt,et al.  Contrast sensitivity as a function of position on the retina , 1980, Vision Research.

[11]  P. Stratton Psychobiology of the Human Newborn , 1982 .

[12]  David K. A. Barnes,et al.  correction: Early visual experience and face processing , 2001, Nature.

[13]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  On the reasons for newborns' responses to faces , 1990 .

[14]  Stimulus energy does not account for 2-month-olds' face preferences. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  K. A. Kleiner Amplitude and phase spectra as indices of infants' pattern preferences. , 1986 .

[16]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Configural Processing at Birth: Evidence for Perceptual Organisation , 2000, Perception.

[17]  R. L. Fantz,et al.  VISUAL PERCEPTION FROM BIRTH AS SHOWN BY PATTERN SELECTIVITY * , 1965, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition , 1993 .

[19]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Leslie B. Cohen,et al.  Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. , 1972, Child development.

[21]  A. Slater,et al.  Newborn Infants' Visual Responses to Square Wave Gratings. , 1977 .

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Role of the Superior Colliculus and Pretectum in Vision and Visually Guided Behavior , 1973 .

[23]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  C. Umilta,et al.  Face preference at birth. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Mark H. Johnson Cortical Maturation and the Development of Visual Attention in Early Infancy , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J S Werner,et al.  Linear systems analysis of infant visual pattern preferences. , 1983, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  A. Ginsburg,et al.  Infant visual preferences: a review and new theoretical treatment. , 1985, Advances in child development and behavior.

[28]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[29]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Possible blindsight in infants lacking one cerebral hemisphere , 1992, Nature.

[30]  A. Slater,et al.  Pattern preferences at birth and their interaction with habituation-induced novelty preferences. , 1985, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  F. Simion,et al.  The origins of face perception: specific versus non‐specific mechanisms , 2001 .

[32]  S. Heywood,et al.  Structure of the Visual Array and Saccadic Latency: Implications for Oculomotor Control* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  K. A. Kleiner,et al.  Stimulus energy does not account for 2-month-olds' face preferences. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[35]  D. Maurer,et al.  Face Perception During Early Infancy , 1999 .

[36]  D. Maurer,et al.  Neuroperception: Early visual experience and face processing , 2001, Nature.

[37]  Leslie B. Cohen,et al.  A Two Process Model of Infant Visual Attention. , 1972 .

[38]  J. Bullinaria,et al.  Reviews: The Race for Consciousness, Fleeting Memories: Cognition of Brief Visual Stimuli, Evolving Thoughts on Thought and its Evolution, An Anatomy of Thought: The Origin and Machinery of the Mind , 2000 .

[39]  C. C. Goren,et al.  Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. , 1975, Pediatrics.