Neural Mechanisms of Post-error Adjustments of Decision Policy in Parietal Cortex

Humans often slow down after mistakes (post-error slowing [PES]), but the neural mechanism and adaptive role of PES remain controversial. We studied changes in the neural mechanisms of decision making after errors in humans and monkeys that performed a motion direction discrimination task. We found that PES is mediated by two factors: a reduction in sensitivity to sensory information and an increase in the decision bound. Both effects are implemented through dynamic changes in the decision-making process. Neuronal responses in the monkey lateral intraparietal area revealed that bound changes are implemented by decreasing an evidence-independent urgency signal. They also revealed a reduction in the rate of evidence accumulation, reflecting reduced sensitivity. These changes in the bound and sensitivity provide a quantitative account of choices and response times. We suggest that PES reflects an adaptive increase of decision bound in anticipation of maladaptive reductions in sensitivity to incoming evidence.

[1]  Elisa Ciaramelli,et al.  The Regulation of Cognitive Control following Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Lesion in Humans , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Naomi Ehrich Leonard,et al.  Can Post-Error Dynamics Explain Sequential Reaction Time Patterns? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[3]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Effects of Cortical Microstimulation on Confidence in a Perceptual Decision , 2014, Neuron.

[4]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.

[5]  Roozbeh Kiani,et al.  A neural mechanism of speed-accuracy tradeoff in macaque area LIP , 2014, eLife.

[6]  A. Fuchs,et al.  A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. , 1966, Journal of applied physiology.

[7]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[9]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[10]  Christopher J. Cueva,et al.  Dynamics of Neural Population Responses in Prefrontal Cortex Indicate Changes of Mind on Single Trials , 2014, Current Biology.

[11]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. III. Memory-contingent visual and saccade responses. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[14]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Neural Basis of Adaptive Response Time Adjustment during Saccade Countermanding , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  L. Selen,et al.  Deliberation in the Motor System: Reflex Gains Track Evolving Evidence Leading to a Decision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[17]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[18]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[19]  Carolin Dudschig,et al.  Short Article: Why do we slow down after an error? Mechanisms underlying the effects of posterror slowing , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  James F. Cavanagh,et al.  Common medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  R. Andersen,et al.  Memory related motor planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque , 1988, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Hans S Schroder,et al.  Improving the study of error monitoring with consideration of behavioral performance measures , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoff by a control signal with balanced excitation and inhibition. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Joachim Vandekerckhove,et al.  A diffusion model account of age differences in posterror slowing. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[28]  J. Kaas,et al.  Supplementary eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation: Connections in macaques , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  A. Pouget,et al.  The Cost of Accumulating Evidence in Perceptual Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Laming Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .

[31]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Improved Probabilistic Inference as a General Learning Mechanism with Action Video Games , 2010, Current Biology.

[32]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[33]  A. Pouget,et al.  Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.

[34]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[36]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Post-Error Behavioral Adjustments Are Facilitated by Activation and Suppression of Task-Relevant and Task-Irrelevant Information Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Ullsperger,et al.  Post-Error Adjustments , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[39]  B. Burle,et al.  The modulation of the Ne-like wave on correct responses foreshadows errors , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[40]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Elapsed Decision Time Affects the Weighting of Prior Probability in a Perceptual Decision Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[42]  Joseph A. Sergeant,et al.  What happens after a hyperactive child commits an error? , 1988, Psychiatry Research.

[43]  Carolin Dudschig,et al.  Speeding before and slowing after errors: Is it all just strategy? , 2009, Brain Research.

[44]  Wim Fias,et al.  Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.

[45]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[46]  Christopher J. Cueva,et al.  Natural Grouping of Neural Responses Reveals Spatially Segregated Clusters in Prearcuate Cortex , 2015, Neuron.

[47]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Sensing and Reproducing a Time Interval , 2015, Current Biology.

[48]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[49]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[50]  A. Schweikard,et al.  Tracking post-error adaptation in the motor system by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2013, Neuroscience.

[51]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Context-Dependent Urgency Influences Speed–Accuracy Trade-Offs in Decision-Making and Movement Execution , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time , 2014, Neuron.

[53]  Veit Stuphorn,et al.  Role of supplementary eye field in saccade initiation: executive, not direct, control. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Supplementary Eye Field during Visual Search: Salience, Cognitive Control, and Performance Monitoring , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[56]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Payoff Information Biases a Fast Guess Process in Perceptual Decision Making under Deadline Pressure: Evidence from Behavior, Evoked Potentials, and Quantitative Model Comparison , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Errors are foreshadowed in brain potentials associated with action monitoring in cingulate cortex in humans , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[60]  Birte U. Forstmann,et al.  How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simple solution , 2012 .

[61]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Evaluating self-generated decisions in frontal pole cortex of monkeys , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[63]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes , 2002, Neuron.