Sound localization with a preceding distractor.

Experiments explored how a distractor coming from a known location influences the localization of a subsequent sound, both in a classroom and in an anechoic chamber. Listeners localized a target click preceded by a distractor click coming from a location fixed throughout a run of trials (either frontal or lateral). The stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) between distractor and target was relatively long (25-400 ms); control trials presented the target alone. The distractor induced bias and variability in target localization responses even at the longest SOA, with the specific pattern of effects differing between the two rooms. Furthermore, the presence of the distractor caused target responses to be displaced away from the distractor location in that run, even on trials with no distractor. This contextual bias built up anew in each run, over the course of minutes. The different effects illustrate that (a) sound localization is a dynamic process that depends on both the context and on the level of reverberation in the environment, and (b) interactions between sequential sound sources occur on time scales from hundreds of milliseconds to as long as minutes.

[1]  Jan Baan,et al.  Spatial fluctuations in measures for spaciousness , 2001 .

[2]  Klaus Hartung,et al.  Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane. I. Psychoacoustical Data , 2002 .

[3]  S. Carlile,et al.  Systematic distortions of auditory space perception following prolonged exposure to broadband noise. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  D. H. Warren,et al.  The role of visual-auditory “compellingness” in the ventriloquism effect: Implications for transitivity among the spatial senses , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  H Steven Colburn,et al.  The influence of spectral, temporal, and interaural stimulus variations on the precedence effect. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  S. Nishida,et al.  Adaptation in the processing of interaural time differences revealed by the auditory localization aftereffect. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Effect of auditory cuing on azimuthal localization accuracy , 2001 .

[9]  W R Thurlow,et al.  Some determinants of localization-adaptation effects for successive auditory stimuli. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  C Trahiotis,et al.  Extents of laterality and binaural interference effects. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  D. P. Phillips,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for adaptation of central auditory processors for interaural differences in time and level , 2005, Hearing Research.

[12]  D R Perrott,et al.  Minimum audible angle thresholds for broadband noise as a function of the delay between the onset of the lead and lag signals. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  Brad Rakerd,et al.  Localization of sound in reverberant spaces , 1999 .

[14]  R. Gilkey,et al.  Sound localization in noise: the effect of signal-to-noise ratio. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  T. Strybel,et al.  Minimum audible angles in the horizontal and vertical planes: effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and burst duration. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  T C Yin,et al.  Physiological studies of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus of the cat. II. Neural mechanisms. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  G. Recanzone Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: the ventriloquism aftereffect. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  D. M. Green,et al.  Sound localization by human listeners. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Comparison of relative and absolute sound localization ability in humans. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  W M Hartmann,et al.  On the minimum audible angle--a decision theory approach. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  B Kollmeier,et al.  Binaural forward and backward masking: evidence for sluggishness in binaural detection. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  R A Reale,et al.  Directional sensitivity of neurons in the primary auditory (AI) cortex of the cat to successive sounds ordered in time and space. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  W Noble,et al.  Available response choices affect localization of sound , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  C Trahiotis,et al.  Peripheral auditory processing and investigations of the "precedence effect" which utilize successive transient stimuli. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[26]  V. Ralph Algazi,et al.  An adaptable ellipsoidal head model for the interaural time difference , 1999, 1999 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing. Proceedings. ICASSP99 (Cat. No.99CH36258).

[27]  B. Delgutte,et al.  Neural correlates of the precedence effect in the inferior colliculus: effect of localization cues. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  W R Thurlow,et al.  Effects of degree of visual association and angle of displacement on the "ventriloquism" effect. , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.

[29]  Patrick M. Zurek,et al.  The Precedence Effect , 1987 .

[30]  R. Litovsky,et al.  Sound localization precision under conditions of the precedence effect: effects of azimuth and standard stimuli. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Location Coding by Opponent Neural Populations in the Auditory Cortex , 2005, PLoS biology.

[32]  T. Yin,et al.  Physiological correlates of the precedence effect and summing localization in the inferior colliculus of the cat , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  Barbara G Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Localizing nearby sound sources in a classroom: binaural room impulse responses. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  H S Colburn,et al.  The precedence effect. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  Daniel J. Tollin,et al.  COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF THE LATERALISATION OF CLICKS AND THEIR ECHOES , 1998 .

[36]  A. Mills On the minimum audible angle , 1958 .

[37]  R. A. Butler,et al.  Some Effects of Unilateral Auditory Masking upon the Localization of Sound in Space , 1962 .

[38]  D C Fitzpatrick,et al.  Responses of neurons to click-pairs as simulated echoes: auditory nerve to auditory cortex. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.