thE EffEcts of constrUal lEvEls on asymmEtric tEmPtation-goal cognitivE associations

Self-control benefits from an asymmetric pattern of cognitive associations whereby temptations facilitate the activation of goals, but goals interfere with the activation of temptations (Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2003). The present research examines how these cognitive associations are dependent on people's subjective construals of events. Drawing from past work demonstrating that abstract, goal-relevant (higher level) rather than concrete, goal-incidental (lower level) construals promote self-control (Fujita, Trope et al., 2006), three experiments tested the hypothesis that higher level construals promote asymmetric temptation-goal associations. Results confirmed that asymmetric temptation-goal associations are construal-dependent: higher level construals enhanced the tendency for temptations to facilitate activation of goals (Studies 1 and 2), and for goals to interfere with the activation of temptation (Study 3).

[1]  P. Gollwitzer Action phases and mind-sets , 1990 .

[2]  G. Loewenstein Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior , 1996 .

[3]  J. Hilton,et al.  Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  R. Wiers,et al.  Personalizing the alcohol-IAT with individualized stimuli: relationship with drinking behavior and drinking-related problems. , 2007, Addictive behaviors.

[5]  K. Fujita,et al.  Influencing Attitudes Toward Near and Distant Objects. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[6]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal Levels and Psychological Distance: Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation, and Behavior. , 2007, Journal of consumer psychology : the official journal of the Society for Consumer Psychology.

[7]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. , 1990 .

[8]  Nira Liberman,et al.  Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: consequences for insight and creative cognition. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  M. Ferguson On the automatic evaluation of end-states. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  L. Ross,et al.  Subjective Construal, Social Inference, and Human Misunderstanding , 1991 .

[11]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Michael A. Olson,et al.  Implicit measures in social cognition. research: their meaning and use. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[13]  Laurie B. Mintz,et al.  Prevalence and correlates of eating disordered behaviors among undergraduate women. , 1988 .

[14]  B. Ostafin,et al.  Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction , 2006 .

[15]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Behavior as seen by the actor and as seen by the observer , 1973 .

[16]  W. Mischel,et al.  A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. , 1995, Psychological review.

[17]  Wen-Hsiung Wu,et al.  Think abstractly, smoke less: a brief construal-level intervention can promote self-control, leading to reduced cigarette consumption among current smokers. , 2013, Addiction.

[18]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal-level theory of psychological distance. , 2010, Psychological review.

[19]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Toward Cigarette Smoking: the Effects of Context and Motivation , 2003 .

[20]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  K. Vohs,et al.  Self-affirmation and self-control: affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Peter Salovey,et al.  Construing action abstractly and blurring social distinctions: implications for perceiving homogeneity among, but also empathizing with and helping, others. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  J. Lindenmeyer Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction: (Eds.). (2006). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. , 2007 .

[24]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[25]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[26]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  Promoting prospective self-control through abstraction ☆ , 2010 .

[27]  H. Cantril,et al.  They saw a game: a case study. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[28]  Karen Gasper,et al.  Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.

[29]  Y. Trope,et al.  Cognitive consequences of affirming the self: The relationship between self-affirmation and object construal. , 2009, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[30]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: memory retrieval as a model case. , 1995, Psychological review.

[31]  Ayelet Fishbach,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Self-Control in Action : Implicit Dispositions Toward Goals and Away From Temptations , 2006 .

[32]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior , 2007 .

[33]  L. Wheeler,et al.  Review of personality and social psychology , 1980 .

[34]  R. Thaler,et al.  An Economic Theory of Self-Control , 1977, Journal of Political Economy.

[35]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  W. Mischel,et al.  When Asking “Why” Does Not Hurt Distinguishing Rumination From Reflective Processing of Negative Emotions , 2005, Psychological science.

[37]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. , 2002 .

[38]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  Inhibition in Goal Systems: A Retrieval-Induced Forgetting Account. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[39]  W. Stroebe,et al.  Pleasure in the mind: Restrained eating and spontaneous hedonic thoughts about food. , 2007 .

[40]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Construal levels and self-control. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  Kentaro Fujita,et al.  Moving Beyond Deliberative Control of Impulses , 2009, Psychological science.

[42]  Richard P. Eibach,et al.  Seeing meaning in action: a bidirectional link between visual perspective and action identification level. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[43]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Leading us not unto temptation: momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  W. Mischel,et al.  Comparative effects of the reward stimulus and its cognitive representation in voluntary delay. , 1976 .

[45]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  www.ssoar.info Why dieters fail: testing the goal conflict model of eating , 2018 .

[46]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. , 1989 .

[47]  W. Stroebe,et al.  Healthy Cognition: Processes of Self-Regulatory Success in Restrained Eating , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[48]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. , 2000 .

[49]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[50]  W. Mischel,et al.  Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. , 1975 .

[51]  Abstract and concrete self-evaluative goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[52]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[53]  J. Bruner On perceptual readiness. , 1957, Psychological review.

[54]  R. Wiers,et al.  Implicitly positive about alcohol? Implicit positive associations predict drinking behavior. , 2008, Addictive behaviors.

[55]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Forgetting all else: on the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Temporal construal. , 2003, Psychological review.

[57]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation Failure: An Overview , 1996 .

[58]  Y. Trope,et al.  The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others' self-regulatory efforts ☆ , 2004 .

[59]  Rui Zhu,et al.  The Influence of Ceiling Height: The Effect of Priming on the Type of Processing That People Use , 2007 .

[60]  H. Rachlin The Science of Self-Control , 2004 .