Cerebrally Lateralized Mental Representations of Hand Shape and Movement

Previous psychophysical and neuroimaging studies suggest that perceiving the handedness of a visually presented hand depends on sensorimotor processes that are specific to the limb of the stimulus and that may be controlled by the cerebral hemisphere contralateral to the limb. Therefore, it was hypothesized that disconnection between cerebral hemispheres would disrupt mental simulation of a hand presented to the ipsilateral, but not the contralateral, hemisphere. This hypothesis was examined by the present study in which two callosotomy patients and eight healthy controls judged the handedness of drawings of left and right hands in various positions, without moving or inspecting their own hands. Stimuli were presented for 150 msec in the right or left visual hemifield. As predicted, for each hemisphere, patients’ accuracy was high when the hand was contralateral to the perceiving hemisphere, but it was not above chance when it was ipsilateral to the perceiving hemisphere. Controls’ accuracy was high in both conditions. Response time analyses indicate patients, like controls, mentally simulated reaching into stimulus postures. When the stimulus laterality was ipsilateral to the perceiving hemisphere, patients imagined the hand contralateral to the perceiving hemisphere reaching into the stimulus posture but did not detect the mismatch, guessing with a response bias or responding on the basis of shape similarity. We conclude that each hemisphere could represent the shape and movement of the contralateral hand but could not for the ipsilateral hand. Mentally simulating one’s action and discriminating body part handedness both depend on lateralized sensorimotor and somatosensory representations.

[1]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[3]  J. Sergent,et al.  Imagery in a commissurotomized patient , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  H. Gastaut,et al.  Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain , 1954 .

[5]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  The nature of hemispheric specialization in man , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[6]  J. Levy Language, cognition, and the right hemisphere. A response to Gazzaniga. , 1983, The American psychologist.

[7]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[8]  D. Kimura,et al.  Acquisition of a motor skill after left-hemisphere damage. , 1977, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  G. Ratcliff Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Memory Encoding Following Complete Callosotomy , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Tarr Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  D. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for motor sequencing: Abnormalities in levels of programming , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  E. Rolls High-level vision: Object recognition and visual cognition, Shimon Ullman. MIT Press, Bradford (1996), ISBN 0 262 21013 4 , 1997 .

[16]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[18]  Jeffrey D. Holtzman,et al.  Interactions between cortical and subcortical visual areas: Evidence from human commissurotomy patients , 1984, Vision Research.

[19]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Book Review Nisbett, R. , & Ross, L.Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment.Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1980. , 1982 .

[20]  J. Klayman,et al.  Confirmation, Disconfirmation, and Informa-tion in Hypothesis Testing , 1987 .

[21]  Michael S. Gazzaniga CEREBRAL MECHANISMS INVOLVED IN IPSILATERAL EYE-HAND USE IN SPLIT-BRAIN MONKEYS. , 1964, Experimental neurology.

[22]  Peter T. Fox,et al.  The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape , 1998 .

[23]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Hypothesis Evaluation from a Bayesian Perspective. , 1983 .

[24]  M. Jeannerod Mental imagery in the motor context , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  A. Benton Laterality: Functional Asymmetry in the Intact Brain. , 1984 .

[26]  L. Ross,et al.  Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. , 1981 .

[27]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Scene-based and viewer-centered representations for comparing shapes , 1988, Cognition.

[28]  L M Parsons,et al.  Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Evidence of foveal splitting in a commissurotomy patient , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Laterality: Functional Asymmetry in the Intact Brain , 1982 .

[32]  I. T. Draper Human Neuropsychology , 1979 .

[33]  R. Ivry,et al.  The two sides of perception , 1997 .

[34]  R. Yin,et al.  Face recognition by brain-injured patients: a dissociable ability? , 1970, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Variability in right hemisphere language function after callosal section: evidence for a continuum of generative capacity , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  M. Corballis,et al.  The Psychology of Left and Right , 2020 .

[37]  M. Coltheart Hemispheric asymmetry , 1978, Nature.

[38]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  MRI assessment of human callosal surgery with neuropsychological correlates , 1985, Neurology.

[39]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Splitbrain Monkeys: Cerebral Control of Ipsilateral and Contralateral Arm, Hand, and Finger Movements , 1972, Science.

[40]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Motor imagery of a lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed in hemi-Parkinson's patients , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[42]  M. Studdert-Kennedy,et al.  Primate handedness reconsidered , 1987, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[43]  Y Agid,et al.  Congruent unilateral impairments for real and imagined hand movements , 1995, Neuroreport.

[44]  C. Hamilton,et al.  Complementary hemispheric specialization in monkeys. , 1988, Science.

[45]  Bruce T. Volpe Cortical mechanisms involved in praxis: observations following partial and complete section of the corpus callosum in man , 1995 .

[46]  J Decety,et al.  Sensation of effort and duration of mentally executed actions. , 1991, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[47]  A. Taylor,et al.  The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition. , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[48]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .

[49]  Andrew C. Papanicolaou,et al.  Visuospatial tasks compared via activation of regional cerebral blood flow , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  R.N.Dej.,et al.  Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain , 1954, Neurology.

[51]  D. Crammond Motor imagery: never in your wildest dream , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[52]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Dyspraxia following division of the cerebral commissures. , 1967, Archives of neurology.

[53]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Organization of the human brain. , 1989, Science.