Some observations on control by spoken words in children's conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion.

This study examined matching-to-sample procedures that might result in the emergence of conditional behavior never explicitly taught. Subjects were preschool children. Two pictures were displayed as comparisons on every trial, and samples were spoken words. In baseline training preceding each of three experiments, children learned to select pictures of a dog, a table, and a banana in response to their spoken English names. Thereafter, probe trials displayed novel comparisons with baseline comparisons: one novel comparison was displayed with the dog and another with the table. The three experiments differed primarily in the nature of the samples presented on probe trials. In Experiment 1, probe samples were novel words, "JAIJAI" and "BREEL." On the probes, each of seven subjects reliably selected the novel comparisons, apparently "excluding" the familiar ones. In Experiment 2, probe samples were from the subjects' baseline. On one probe, for example, the sample was "TABLE," and the subject had to choose either the dog or the novel picture. Exclusion was logically possible because the dog had always before been incorrect in the presence of "TABLE." Under these conditions, however, only two of nine children excluded reliably. In Experiment 3, probe samples were words that had never been samples on any matching-to-sample trial, but that had controlled the children's behavior in other settings. On one probe, for example, the sample was "PENCIL," and the subject had to choose either the dog or a novel picture. Subjects virtually always excluded the former and selected the latter. Unreliable exclusion in Experiment 2, therefore, apparently resulted because the probe samples had previously served also as samples on baseline trials. Spontaneous verbalizations recorded during probing provided further data consistent with this interpretation. The study helps to define variables controlling exclusion performances by showing that such performances are more likely to occur if the sample has no prior experimental history.

[1]  R. Stromer,et al.  Control of adolescents' arbitrary matching-to-sample by positive and negative stimulus relations. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[2]  L. S. Dixon,et al.  The nature of control by spoken words over visual stimulus selection. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[4]  D E Carter,et al.  Complex learning and information processing by pigeons: a critical analysis. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  L. S. Dixon,et al.  The nature of standard control in children's matching-to-sample. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[6]  M. Sidman A note on the measurement of conditional discrimination. , 1980, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  W. McIlvane,et al.  Positive and negative stimulus relations in severely retarded individuals′ conditional discrimination , 1984 .

[8]  B. J. Gerovac,et al.  Spoken and signed naming of foods after receptive exclusion training in severe retardation. , 1984, Applied research in mental retardation.

[9]  B. Skinner Contingencies of reinforcement : a theoretical analysis , 1969 .

[10]  W J McIlvane,et al.  Acquisition of matching - to - sample performances in severe retardation: learning by exclusion. , 1981, Journal of mental deficiency research.

[11]  W. McIlvane,et al.  Complex stimulus relations and exclusion in severe mental retardation , 1985 .

[12]  J. D. de Rose,et al.  Controlling relations in conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. , 1987, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  Control by exclusion in arbitrary matching-to-sample , 1986 .

[14]  L. Vincent-Smith,et al.  Acquisition of receptive vocabulary in the toddler-age child. , 1974, Child development.