The effect of perceptual distinctiveness on the prospective and retrospective components of prospective memory in young and old adults.

In two experiments, the effect of perceptual distinctiveness of cues on prospective memory performance was examined. Young and older adults completed a visual search task with embedded prospective memory instructions. On each trial, participants were asked to indicate the position of a target letter in a letter string, unless either of two letters previously identified as prospective memory cues was presented. Each prospective cue was associated with a specific response. Perceptual distinctiveness was manipulated by spatially displacing a single letter. The prospective component (successful detection of the cue) and the retrospective memory component (recalling the correct response when a cue is detected) were measured separately. Perceptual displacement of cues modulated performance of the prospective component but not the retrospective component. Young adults successfully detected a larger proportion of cues (prospective component) than older adults. However, there were minimal effects of age and no effect of cue displacement on participants' ability to recall the intention once they detected a cue (retrospective component performance). Results are discussed within the context of current theoretical models of prospective memory.

[1]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .

[2]  M. Conway Cognitive models of memory , 1997 .

[3]  F. Craik,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 1992 .

[4]  S. L. Richardson,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Age-related deficits in prospective memory: the influence of task complexity. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[6]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Memory Change in the Aged , 1998 .

[7]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Habitual Prospective Memory and Aging: Remembering Intentions and Forgetting Actions , 1998 .

[8]  E. Clark,et al.  Is perceptual salience needed in explanations of the isolation effect? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Rebekah E. Smith The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  R. West,et al.  Event‐related neural activity associated with prospective remembering , 2000 .

[11]  D. B. Mitchell,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 2001 .

[12]  R. Dixon,et al.  Episodic memory change in late adulthood: Generalizability across samples and performance indices , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[13]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework , 2000 .

[14]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptually driven processes? , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Maria Larsson,et al.  Cognitive functioning in very old age , 1999 .

[16]  Roger W. Morrell,et al.  Processing of Medical information in Aging Patients : Cognitive and Human Factors Perspectives , 1999 .

[17]  F. Craik,et al.  Age-related decline in prospective memory: the roles of cue accessibility and cue sensitivity. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[18]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: forgetting intentions over short delays. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[19]  P. Rendell,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: differences between naturalistic and laboratory tasks. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[20]  J. Ellis Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. , 1996 .

[21]  Géry d'Ydewalle,et al.  The importance of on-going concurrent activities as a function of age in time- and event-based prospective memory , 1999 .

[22]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Changes in Event-Based Prospective Memory Across Adulthood , 1998 .

[23]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995 .

[24]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  J. Ward,et al.  Data-driven recognition memory: A new technique and some data on age differences , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Betula: A Prospective Cohort Study on Memory, Health and Aging , 2004 .

[27]  R. Dixon,et al.  Characteristics of Self-Reported Memory Compensation in Older Adults , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[28]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  R. West,et al.  Neural activity associated with the realization of a delayed intention. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[30]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Does prospective memory decline with age , 1996 .

[31]  Anna-Lisa Cohen,et al.  Modulation of the Prospective and Retrospective Components of Memory for Intentions in Younger and Older Adults , 2001 .

[32]  E. Maylor,et al.  Age-related impairment in an event-based prospective-memory task. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[33]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Ask and ye shall receive : Behavioural specificity in the accuracy of subjective memory complaints , 2000 .

[34]  J. Cerella,et al.  Adult information processing: Limits on loss. , 1993 .

[35]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The relationship between prospective memory and retrospective memory: neuropsychological evidence , 1997 .

[36]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Minimized prospective memory loss in old age , 1993 .