Abnormal fMRI connectivity in children with dyslexia during a phoneme task: Before but not after treatment

Brains of 18 children with dyslexia (5 girls, 13 boys) and 21 and without dyslexia (8 girls, 13 boys) were scanned before and after the children with dyslexia received instructional treatment. Both at Time 1 and Time 2 all children performed an fMRI phoneme mapping task during brain scanning-deciding whether letter(s) in pair of pronounceable nonwords could stand for the same sound. Results were analyzed with a seed point correlational method for functional connectivity from four seed points based on prior studies: inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, the occipital region, and cerebellum. At Time 1 before treatment, a significant difference in fMRI connectivity occurred between children with dyslexia and normal reading controls in the left inferior frontal gyrus and its correlations with right and left middle frontal gyrus, right and left supplemental motor area, left precentral gyrus, and right superior frontal gyrus. There were no significant differences for the seed regions placed in the middle frontal gyrus, occipital gyrus or cerebellum. Children with dyslexia had greater functional connectivity from the left inferior frontal gyrus seed point to the right inferior frontal gyrus than did the children without dyslexia. Compared to adults with and without dyslexia who differed in bilateral connectivity from left inferior frontal gyrus on the same task, the children with and without dyslexia differed in left side connectivity from left inferior frontal gyrus. At Time 2 after treatment, the children with dyslexia, who had participated in a three-week instructional program that provided explicit instruction in linguistic awareness, alphabetic principle (taught in a way to maximize temporal contiguity of grapheme-phoneme associations), decoding and spelling, and a writers' workshop, did not differ from the children without dyslexia in any of the clusters in the group difference map identifying differences between dyslexics and good readers, showing that functional connectivity (and not just regions of interest) may normalize following instructional treatment.

[1]  John C Gore,et al.  Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[2]  Charles A. MacArthur,et al.  Handbook of Writing Research , 2005 .

[3]  D. Cordes,et al.  Low-frequency signal changes reflect differences in functional connectivity between good readers and dyslexics during continuous phoneme mapping. , 2006, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[4]  Gary J. Robertson,et al.  Wide Range Achievement Test 3 ( wrat3 ) , 2001 .

[5]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[6]  A. Liberman,et al.  Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  W. Nagy,et al.  Individual fMRI activation in orthographic mapping and morpheme mapping after orthographic or morphological spelling treatment in child dyslexics , 2006, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[8]  S C Cramer,et al.  Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia , 2003, Neurology.

[9]  J. Mitchell Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing , 2001 .

[10]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  The cerebellum's role in reading: a functional MR imaging study. , 1999, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[11]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[12]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: a magnetic source imaging approach. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  Karen M. Jones,et al.  Neural Changes following Remediation in Adult Developmental Dyslexia , 2004, Neuron.

[15]  R. Venezky The Structure of English Orthography , 1965 .

[16]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Shifts of Effective Connectivity within a Language Network during Rhyming and Spelling , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Z. Breznitz Asynchrony of visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological word recognition processes: An underlying factor in dyslexia , 2002 .

[18]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A Three-Dimensional Statistical Analysis for CBF Activation Studies in Human Brain , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[19]  E. Wijsman,et al.  Modeling Phonological Core Deficits Within a Working Memory Architecture in Children and Adults With Developmental Dyslexia , 2006 .

[20]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[21]  Wendy Raskind,et al.  Converging Evidence for Triple Word Form Theory in Children With Dyslexia , 2006, Developmental neuropsychology.

[22]  Zvia Breznitz,et al.  Fluency in Reading: Synchronization of Processes , 2005 .

[23]  M. Eckert,et al.  Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: frontal and cerebellar findings. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  Virginia W. Berninger,et al.  Brain literacy for educators and psychologists , 2002 .

[25]  W. Nagy,et al.  Brain activation during language task contrasts in children with and without dyslexia: Inferring mapping processes and assessing response to spelling instruction , 2005, Educational and Child Psychology.

[26]  Maryanne Wolf,et al.  Rapid alternating stimulus naming in the developmental dyslexias , 1986, Brain and Language.

[27]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference , 2004, NeuroImage.

[28]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[29]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Patricia Greig Bowers,et al.  Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia , 1993 .

[31]  B. Shaywitz,et al.  A definition of dyslexia , 2003 .

[32]  A. Liberman,et al.  The Angular Gyrus in Developmental Dyslexia: Task-Specific Differences in Functional Connectivity Within Posterior Cortex , 2000, Psychological science.

[33]  Virginia W. Berninger,et al.  Language Phenotype for Reading and Writing Disability: A Family Approach , 2001 .

[34]  B. Horwitz,et al.  Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[36]  Robert D Abbott,et al.  Reproducibility of proton MR spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI): comparison of dyslexic and normal-reading children and effects of treatment on brain lactate levels during language tasks. , 2002, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[37]  M. Wolf,et al.  Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: a longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. , 1986, Child development.

[38]  Virginia W. Berninger,et al.  Tier 3 specialized writing instruction for students with dyslexia , 2008 .

[39]  M. Lowe,et al.  Functional Connectivity in Single and Multislice Echoplanar Imaging Using Resting-State Fluctuations , 1998, NeuroImage.

[40]  C. Hlaing,et al.  An Interdisciplinary Journal , 2009 .

[41]  S. Cichon,et al.  Evidence for linkage of spelling disability to chromosome 15. , 1998, American journal of human genetics.