The Effects of Positive and Negative Mood on Cognition and Motivation in Multimedia Learning Environment

Introduction In a typical e-learning and computer-based learning environment, learners are confronted with information presented in the format of text, narration, animation, and graphics. Hence, learners engage in the cognitive process known as multimedia learning (Mayer, 2008). The cognitive process of multimedia learning describes an information processing in which learners select relevant visual and verbal materials, organize these visual and verbal mental representations in coherent structures in working memory, and integrate the visual and the verbal mental representations with one another and with prior knowledge (Mayer, 2008). Complementing this process model, the cognitive load theory (CLT) provides a capacity model of multimedia learning (Kalyuga, 2011). The CLT model highlights the inherent limitation of working memory and describes three types of cognitive load: intrinsic, germane, and extraneous. Intrinsic cognitive load is the inherent level of difficulty associated with a specific instructional topic. Germane load defines the amount of mental effort invested by the learner in comprehending the materials, whereas extraneous load describes processing demands of information that are not directly related to the learning task (Kalyuga, 2011). Multimedia learning design has been primarily based on cognitive models such as dual-coding theory and cognitive load theory (Mayer & Moreno, 2003). However, Moreno's (2006) Cognitive-Affective Theory of Learning with Media extended the framework by integrating emotional and motivational factors in multimedia learning design. According to this framework, the process of multimedia learning is mediated by emotional factors that influence learner's willingness to invest mental effort to the learning process (Park et al., 2015). In addition, the framework assumes that multimedia learning process is mediated by learner's metacognition such as cognitive styles and abilities (Moreno & Duran, 2004; Park et al., 2015). Hence, a learner's working memory, motivation (i.e., germane load), and cognitive style is affected by his or her emotional states. Recent studies have attempted to validate the importance of emotional factors in the design of multimedia learning environments. In a seminal work by Um et al. (2012), it was demonstrated that learners who were induced with positive mood outperformed learners who were induced with neutral mood in comprehension and transfer tests. The researchers also showed that positive emotion can be transferred to learners through interface designs, such as anthropomorphism and bright colors. In the attempt to replicate these findings, Plass et al. (2014) tested the effects of color and anthropomorphism on learners' emotion, motivational and cognitive outcomes. It was shown that positive emotional design in multimedia learning environments enhanced positive emotion, motivation, and comprehension. Moreover, positive emotional design in a multimedia interface significantly reduced learners' perception of task difficulty. However, the study failed to replicate the facilitating effect of positive emotional design on transfer scores. Mayer and Estrella (2014) tested the effects of positive emotional design in multimedia learning environments. Mirroring earlier findings (Plass et al., 2014; Um et al., 2012;), results from their experiments showed that positive emotional design via anthropomorphism and appealing colors enhanced retention scores. In an eye tracking experiment, Park et al. (2015) revealed that learners who were induced with positive mood had higher learning outcomes in comprehension and transfer tests and showed longer fixation durations on the relevant information of the multimedia learning environment than learners who were induced with neutral mood. Their findings empirically indicate that emotional state influences cognitive and attentional processing in multimedia learning. Moods in multimedia learning environment There are different constructs when defining "emotion" (Ekkekakis, 2012; Russell, 2003). …

[1]  Karl-Heinz Bäuml,et al.  Remembering Can Cause Forgetting—but Not in Negative Moods , 2007, Psychological science.

[2]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[3]  P. Ekkekakis A f f e c t , M o o d , a n d E m o t i o n , 2022 .

[4]  G. Wilkinson,et al.  Gender differences in depression. Critical review. , 2000, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[5]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  R. Mayer Applying the science of learning: evidence-based principles for the design of multimedia instruction. , 2008, The American psychologist.

[7]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Delighted and distracted: positive affect increases priming for irrelevant information. , 2011, Emotion.

[8]  R. Moreno,et al.  Does the modality principle hold for different media? A test of the method-affects-learning hypothesis , 2006, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[9]  Brett A. S. Martin The influence of gender on mood effects in advertising , 2003 .

[10]  Robert A. Sottilare,et al.  Passively Classifying Student Mood and Performance within Intelligent Tutors , 2012, J. Educ. Technol. Soc..

[11]  Tze Wei Liew,et al.  The Effects of Learners' Differences on Variable Manipulation Behaviors in Simulation-Based Learning , 2014 .

[12]  P. Jennings,et al.  Valence and Arousal Ratings for Velten Mood Induction Statements , 2000 .

[13]  R. Mayer,et al.  Benefits of emotional design in multimedia instruction , 2014 .

[14]  H. Ellis,et al.  Irrelevant thoughts, emotional mood states, and cognitive task performance , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[16]  L. Phillips,et al.  The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  C. Carver,et al.  Origins and Functions of Positive and Negative Affect: A Control-Process View. , 1990 .

[19]  E. Velten A laboratory task for induction of mood states. , 1968, Behaviour research and therapy.

[20]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  Gender Differences in Depression , 2001 .

[21]  Sandra W. Russ,et al.  On the Relationship Between Affect and Creative Problem Solving , 2015 .

[22]  Errol Thompson,et al.  Bloom's taxonomy for CS assessment , 2008, ACE '08.

[23]  A. Isen,et al.  The Influence of Positive Affect on Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: Facilitating Enjoyment of Play, Responsible Work Behavior, and Self-Control , 2005 .

[24]  F. Paas,et al.  Effects of the Physical Environment on Cognitive Load and Learning: Towards a New Model of Cognitive Load , 2014 .

[25]  J. Forgas Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  R. Erber,et al.  Beyond mood and social judgment: Mood incongruent recall and mood regulation , 1994 .

[27]  Eunjoon Rachel Um,et al.  Emotional design in multimedia learning. , 2012 .

[28]  J. Russell Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. , 2003, Psychological review.

[29]  R. Mayer,et al.  Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[30]  L. Phillips,et al.  Positive mood and executive function: evidence from stroop and fluency tasks. , 2002, Emotion.

[31]  Clifford Nass,et al.  Emotion in human-computer interaction , 2002 .

[32]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought‐action repertoires , 2005, Cognition & emotion.

[33]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Is seeking bad mood cognitively demanding? Contra-hedonic orientation and working-memory capacity in everyday life. , 2011, Emotion.

[34]  G. Clore,et al.  Feelings and phenomenal experiences , 1996 .

[35]  Klaus Fiedler,et al.  Affective States and the Influence of Activated General Knowledge , 1995 .

[36]  Karen Gasper,et al.  When necessity is the mother of invention: Mood and problem solving , 2003 .

[37]  A. Tesser,et al.  Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis , 1992 .

[38]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Incorporating Learner Experience into the Design of Multimedia Instruction. , 2000 .

[39]  Slava Kalyuga Cognitive Load Theory: How Many Types of Load Does It Really Need? , 2011 .

[40]  J. Forgas Don’t Worry, Be Sad! On the Cognitive, Motivational, and Interpersonal Benefits of Negative Mood , 2013 .

[41]  Julia Müller,et al.  Emotional design in multimedia learning: Differentiation on relevant design features and their effects on emotions and learning , 2015, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[42]  V. Aleven,et al.  Triggering situational interest by decorative illustrations both fosters and hinders learning in computer-based learning environments , 2014 .

[43]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[44]  Roland Brünken,et al.  Emotional design and positive emotions in multimedia learning: An eyetracking study on the use of anthropomorphisms , 2015, Comput. Educ..

[45]  Elizabeth O. Hayward,et al.  Emotional design in multimedia learning: Effects of shape and color on affect and learning , 2014 .

[46]  Nico H. Frijda,et al.  Emotion Experience and its Varieties , 2009 .

[47]  J. Forgas,et al.  Can bad weather improve your memory? An unobtrusive field study of natural mood effects on real-life memory , 2009 .

[48]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Daily variability in working memory is coupled with negative affect: the role of attention and motivation. , 2012, Emotion.

[49]  S. Brand,et al.  How do we learn in a negative mood? Effects of a negative mood on transfer and learning , 2007 .

[50]  R. Moreno,et al.  Do Multiple Representations Need Explanations? The Role of Verbal Guidance and Individual Differences in Multimedia Mathematics Learning , 2004 .

[51]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Rethinking Rumination , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.