The Saccade Main Sequence in Patients With Retinitis Pigmentosa and Advanced Age-Related Macular Degeneration

Purpose Most eye-movement studies in patients with visual field defects have examined the strategies that patients use while exploring a visual scene, but they have not investigated saccade kinematics. In healthy vision, saccade trajectories follow the remarkably stereotyped “main sequence”: saccade duration increases linearly with saccade amplitude; peak velocity also increases linearly for small amplitudes, but approaches a saturation limit for large amplitudes. Recent theories propose that these relationships reflect the brain's attempt to optimize vision when planning eye movements. Therefore, in patients with bilateral retinal damage, saccadic behavior might differ to optimize vision under the constraints imposed by the visual field defects. Methods We compared saccadic behavior of patients with central vision loss, due to age-related macular degeneration (AMD), and patients with peripheral vision loss, due to retinitis pigmentosa (RP), to that of controls with normal vision (NV) using a horizontal saccade task. Results Both patient groups demonstrated deficits in saccade reaction times and target localization behavior, as well as altered saccade kinematics. Saccades were generally slower and the shape of the velocity profiles were often atypical, especially in the patients with RP. In the patients with AMD, the changes were far less dramatic. For both groups, saccade kinematics were affected most when the target was in the subjects’ blind field. Conclusions We conclude that defects of the central and peripheral retina have distinct effects on the saccade main sequence, and that visual inputs play an important role in planning the kinematics of a saccade.

[1]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoffs influence the main sequence of saccadic eye movements , 2022, Scientific Reports.

[2]  P. Verghese,et al.  Eye Movements in Macular Degeneration. , 2021, Annual review of vision science.

[3]  Dan Lindfield,et al.  Using an open-source tablet perimeter (Eyecatcher) as a rapid triage measure for glaucoma clinic waiting areas , 2020, British Journal of Ophthalmology.

[4]  Erwan J. David,et al.  Predicting artificial visual field losses: A gaze-based inference study. , 2019, Journal of vision.

[5]  Samuel A. Titchener,et al.  Head and Gaze Behavior in Retinitis Pigmentosa. , 2019, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[6]  Irene Schmidtmann,et al.  Age dependent normative data of vertical and horizontal reflexive saccades , 2018, PloS one.

[7]  E. Salinas,et al.  Saccade metrics reflect decision-making dynamics during urgent choices , 2018, Nature Communications.

[8]  David J. Mack,et al.  The effect of sampling rate and lowpass filters on saccades – A modeling approach , 2017, Behavior Research Methods.

[9]  S. McKee,et al.  Saccadic latency in amblyopia , 2016, Journal of vision.

[10]  Marcus Nyström,et al.  Post-saccadic oscillations in eye movement data recorded with pupil-based eye trackers reflect motion of the pupil inside the iris , 2013, Vision Research.

[11]  B. P. Klein,et al.  Macular degeneration affects eye movement behavior during visual search , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[12]  Usha Chakravarthy,et al.  Clinical classification of age-related macular degeneration. , 2013, Ophthalmology.

[13]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Tracking the eye non-invasively: simultaneous comparison of the scleral search coil and optical tracking techniques in the macaque monkey , 2012, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[14]  A. John van Opstal,et al.  Optimal Control of Saccades by Spatial-Temporal Activity Patterns in the Monkey Superior Colliculus , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[15]  Thomas R. Reppert,et al.  Changes in saccade kinematics associated with the value and novelty of a stimulus , 2012, 2012 46th Annual Conference on Information Sciences and Systems (CISS).

[16]  Cornelius Weber,et al.  Learning the Optimal Control of Coordinated Eye and Head Movements , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[17]  Peter J. Bex,et al.  Effects of Peripheral Visual Field Loss on Eye Movements During Visual Search , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[18]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Differential effects of reflex blinks on saccade perturbations in humans. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Adonis K Moschovakis,et al.  Optimal Control of Gaze Shifts , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  The intrinsic value of visual information affects saccade velocities , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Gang Luo,et al.  The role of peripheral vision in saccade planning: learning from people with tunnel vision. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[22]  R. T. Smith,et al.  PREFERRED RETINAL LOCUS IN MACULAR DISEASE: Characteristics and Clinical Implications , 2008, Retina.

[23]  A. John van Opstal,et al.  Linear ensemble-coding in midbrain superior colliculus specifies the saccade kinematics , 2008, Biological Cybernetics.

[24]  R. V. van Beers Saccadic Eye Movements Minimize the Consequences of Motor Noise , 2008, PloS one.

[25]  Xintian Hu,et al.  Reliability of oculomotor command signals carried by individual neurons , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  E. Peli,et al.  Eye movements of patients with tunnel vision while walking. , 2006, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[27]  L. Optican,et al.  What clinical disorders tell us about the neural control of saccadic eye movements , 2006 .

[28]  Jeffrey S. Perry,et al.  Visual search: the role of peripheral information measured using gaze-contingent displays. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[29]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  The Main Sequence of Saccades Optimizes Speed-accuracy Trade-off , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.

[30]  Ning Qian,et al.  An optimization principle for determining movement duration. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Ronald A Schuchard,et al.  Preferred retinal loci and macular scotoma characteristics in patients with age-related macular degeneration. , 2005, Canadian journal of ophthalmology. Journal canadien d'ophtalmologie.

[32]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Influence of Artificial Scotomas on Eye Movements during Visual Search , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[33]  C. Kennard,et al.  Using saccades as a research tool in the clinical neurosciences , 2004 .

[34]  Eli Peli,et al.  Scanning Eye Movements in Homonymous Hemianopia Documented by Scanning Laser Ophthalmoscope Retinal Perimetry , 2003, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[35]  K. Turano,et al.  Fixation behavior while walking: persons with central visual field loss , 2002, Vision Research.

[36]  K. Turano,et al.  Direction of Gaze while Walking a Simple Route: Persons with Normal Vision and Persons with Retinitis Pigmentosa , 2001, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[37]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  A J Van Opstal,et al.  Blink-perturbed saccades in monkey. II. Superior colliculus activity. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Primate antisaccades. I. Behavioral characteristics. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[41]  J. A. Gisbergen,et al.  Listing's plane dependence on alternating fixation in a strabismus patient , 1997, Vision Research.

[42]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[43]  S. Whittaker,et al.  Saccade control without a fovea , 1991, Vision Research.

[44]  R. Baker,et al.  Discharge characteristics of medial rectus and abducens motoneurons in the goldfish. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  H. Bedell,et al.  The oculomotor reference in humans with bilateral macular disease. , 1990, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[46]  B. Torres,et al.  Analysis of the fluctuations in the interspike intervals of abducens nucleus neurons during ocular fixation in the alert cat , 1986, Brain Research.

[47]  B. Troost,et al.  The effects of age on normal saccadic characteristics and their variability , 1983, Vision Research.

[48]  C. Schor,et al.  Slow control of eye position in strabismic amblyopia. , 1978, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[49]  L. Stark,et al.  Dynamic overshoot in saccadic eye movements is caused by neurological control signal reversals , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[50]  A. J. Van Opstal,et al.  Component stretching in fast and slow oblique saccades in the human , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.