The Developmental Trajectory of Nonadjacent Dependency Learning.

We investigated the developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning in an artificial language. Infants were exposed to 1 of 2 artificial languages with utterances of the form [aXc or bXd] (Grammar 1) or [aXd or bXc] (Grammar 2). In both languages, the grammaticality of an utterance depended on the relation between the 1 st and 3rd elements, whereas the intervening element varied freely. High variability of the middle element is known to contribute to perception of nonadjacent dependencies (Góomez, 2002), but the developmental trajectory of such learning is unknown. Experiment 1 replicated the study of Gómez with a younger age group and a more subtle variability manipulation. Twelve-month-olds failed to track nonadjacent dependencies under conditions tested here (Experiments 2a and 2b), but by 15 months, infants are beginning to track this structure (Experiment 3). Such learning has implications for understanding how infants might begin to acquire similar structure in natural language.

[1]  Zellig S. Harris,et al.  From Phoneme to Morpheme , 1955 .

[2]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[3]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[4]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[5]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  The role of constituent structure in the induction of an artificial language , 1981 .

[6]  E. Newport Constraints on learning and their role in language acquisition: Studies of the acquisition of American sign language , 1988 .

[7]  E. W. Ames,et al.  A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. , 1988 .

[8]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[9]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[10]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.

[11]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[12]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[13]  E. Newport,et al.  Computation of Conditional Probability Statistics by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1998 .

[14]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space , 1998, Cognition.

[15]  L. Gerken,et al.  An electrophysiological study of infants' sensitivity to the sound patterns of English speech. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[16]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[17]  R. Gómez,et al.  Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[18]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  The basis of transfer in artificial grammar learning , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[19]  R. Gómez,et al.  Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy , 2010 .

[21]  Jane B. Childers,et al.  The role of pronouns in young children's acquisition of the English transitive construction. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[22]  Elissa L. Newport,et al.  Innately Constrained Learning: Blending Old and New Approaches to Language Acquisition , 2001 .

[23]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  Marina Nespor,et al.  Signal-Driven Computations in Speech Processing , 2002, Science.

[25]  Toben H. Mintz Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[26]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[27]  J. Saffran,et al.  From Syllables to Syntax: Multilevel Statistical Learning by 12-Month-Old Infants , 2003 .

[28]  Nick Chater,et al.  Reduction of Uncertainty in Human Sequential Learning: Evidence from Artificial Grammar Learning , 2003 .

[29]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Pattern induction by infant language learners. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[30]  Toben H. Mintz Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech , 2003, Cognition.

[31]  E. Newport,et al.  Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  Nick Chater,et al.  Variability is the spice of learning, and a crucial ingredient for detecting and generalizing in nonadjacent dependencies , 2004 .

[33]  R. Peereman,et al.  Learning Nonadjacent Dependencies: No Need for Algebraic-like Computations Is It Possible to Learn the Relation between 2 Nonadjacent Events? , 2004 .

[34]  Nick Chater,et al.  Processing non-adjacent contingencies: A graded, associative account , 2005 .

[35]  Nick Chater,et al.  Phonology impacts segmentation and generalization in speech processing , 2005 .

[36]  Rebecca L. Gómez,et al.  Dynamically guided learning , 2005 .

[37]  Nick Chater,et al.  Phonology impacts segmentation in online speech processing , 2005 .

[38]  J. Weissenborn,et al.  The Recognition of Discontinuous Verbal Dependencies by German 19-Month-Olds: Evidence for Lexical and Structural Influences on Children's Early Processing Capacities , 2006 .