Peripheral constraint versus on-line programming in rapid aimed sequential movements.

The purpose of this investigation was to examine how the programming and control of a rapid aiming sequence shifts with increased complexity. One objective was to determine if a preprogramming/peripheral constraint explanation is adequate to characterize control of an increasingly complex rapid aiming sequence, and if not, at what point on-line programming better accounts for the data. A second objective was to examine when on-line programming occurs. Three experiments were conducted in which complexity was manipulated by increasing the number of targets from 1 to 11. Initiation- and execution-timing patterns, probe reaction time (RT), and movement kinematics were measured. Results supported the peripheral constraint/pre-programming explanation for sequences up to 7 targets if they were executed in a blocked fashion. For sequences executed in a random fashion (one length followed by a different length), preprogramming did not increase with complexity, and on-line programming occurred without time cost. Across all sequences there was evidence that the later targets created a peripheral constraint on movements to previous targets. We suggest that programming is influenced by two factors: the overall spatial trajectory, which is consistent with Sidaway's subtended angle (SA) hypothesis (1991), and average velocity, with the latter established based on the number of targets in the sequence. As the number of targets increases, average velocity decreases, which controls variability of error in the extent of each movement segment. Overall the data support a continuous model of processing, one in which programming and execution co-occur, and can do so without time cost.

[1]  M G Fischman,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Motor Behavior Influence of Extended Practice on Programming Time, Movement Time, and Transfer in Simple Target-striking Responses , 2022 .

[2]  Ian M. Franks,et al.  On-line programming of simple movement sequences , 1997 .

[3]  D. Meyer,et al.  Control of Serial Order in Rapidly Spoken Syllable Sequences , 1987 .

[4]  C J Chamberlin,et al.  Preparation and control of rapid, multisegmented responses in simple and choice environments. , 1989, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[5]  John B. Shea,et al.  A Movement Constraint Interpretation of the Response Complexity Effect on Programming Time , 1988 .

[6]  H. F. Crovitz,et al.  A group-test for assessing hand- and eye-dominance. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[7]  S. Portier,et al.  Practice and the dynamics of handwriting performance: evidence for a shift of motor programming load. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  Hiroshi Sekiya,et al.  Movement variability as a function of accuracy demand in programmed serial aiming responses , 1995 .

[9]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[10]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Scheduling and programming of rapid finger sequences: tests and elaborations of the hierarchical editor model. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  A. T. Welford,et al.  The fundamentals of skill , 1968 .

[12]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Do cortical and basal ganglionic motor areas use “motor programs” to control movement? , 1992 .

[13]  S Kornblum,et al.  Does motor programming necessitate response execution? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  R W Christina,et al.  The 1991 C. H. McCloy Research Lecture: unraveling the mystery of the response complexity effect in skilled movements. , 1992, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[15]  I M Franks,et al.  Preprogramming vs. on-line control in simple movement sequences. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[16]  G. Stelmach Information processing in motor control and learning , 1978 .

[17]  Y. T. Wang,et al.  Cinematographical Analysis of Movement Pathway Constraints in Rapid Target-Striking Tasks. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  Stanley J. Portier,et al.  Immediate vs. postponed visual feedback in practising a handwriting task , 1992 .

[19]  Daniel Russell,et al.  Distributed Control in Rapid Sequential Aiming Responses. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  F. M. Henry,et al.  Increased Response Latency for Complicated Movements and A “Memory Drum” Theory of Neuromotor Reaction , 1960 .

[21]  Andras Semjen,et al.  The organization of rapid movement sequences as a function of sequence length , 1987 .

[22]  A Semjen,et al.  Distributed planning of movement sequences. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  Mg Fischman,et al.  Slower movement times may not necessarily imply online programming , 1990 .

[24]  C J Worringham,et al.  Distribution of Programming in a Rapid Aimed Sequential Movement , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  F M Henry,et al.  Use of simple reaction time in motor programming studies: a reply to Klapp, Wyatt and Lingo. , 1980, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  Howard N. Zelaznik,et al.  Advances in Motor Learning and Control , 1996 .

[27]  Gerald A. Smith,et al.  Padding point extrapolation techniques for the Butterworth digital filter. , 1989, Journal of biomechanics.

[28]  B Sidaway,et al.  Motor programming as a function of constraints on movement initiation. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  A. Gordon,et al.  Choosing between movement sequences: A hierarchical editor model. , 1984 .

[30]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .

[31]  W. Verwey,et al.  Practicing a Structured Continuous Key-Pressing Task: Motor Chunking or Rhythm Consolidation? , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  D W Massaro,et al.  Information processing models: microscopes of the mind. , 1993, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  Ian M. Franks,et al.  Response programming as a function of accuracy and complexity: Evidence from latency and kinematic measures , 1997 .

[34]  S. Klapp,et al.  Response programming in simple and choice reactions. , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.

[35]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting , 1978 .

[36]  B Hyland,et al.  Cortical cell assemblies: a possible mechanism for motor programs. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  W. B. Verwey,et al.  A Forthcoming Key Press Can Be Selected While Earlier Ones Are Executed. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  M G Fischman,et al.  Programming time as a function of number of movement parts and changes in movement direction. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.