Repetition blindness reveals differences between the representations of manipulable and nonmanipulable objects.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Irina M Harris | Sally Andrews | William G Hayward | I. M. Harris | Claire O'Callaghan | S. Andrews | W. Hayward | C. O’Callaghan | Alexandra M. Murray | Alexandra M Murray | I. Harris
[1] P. Jolicoeur,et al. Repetition blindness for pseudoobject pictures. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] L. Buxbaum,et al. Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.
[3] D. Bartram. Levels of coding in picture-picture comparison tasks , 1976, Memory & cognition.
[4] V. Coltheart. Fleeting memories : cognition of brief visual stimuli , 1999 .
[5] Irina M Harris,et al. Repetition blindness for rotated objects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] I. M. Harris,et al. Automatic motor cortex activation for natural as compared to awkward grips of a manipulable object , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] Christopher Barry,et al. Age of Acquisition, Word Frequency, and the Locus of Repetition Priming of Picture Naming , 2001 .
[8] T. R. Jordan,et al. Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[9] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. What is the role of motor simulation in action and object recognition? Evidence from apraxia , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[10] J. Segui,et al. Repetition blindness between words : Nature of the orthographic and phonological representations involved BAVELIER , 2022 .
[11] Sally A. Linkenauger,et al. A Functional Role for Motor Simulation in Identifying Tools , 2010, Psychological science.
[12] G. Humphreys,et al. Recognition by action: dissociating visual and semantic routes to action in normal observers. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] G. Gigli,et al. Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.
[14] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[15] Knowledge as an Object , 2000, Ritual and Mythology of the Chinese Triads.
[16] M. Potter,et al. Time to understand pictures and words , 1975, Nature.
[17] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[18] C. Harris,et al. Repetition blindness occurs in nonwords. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] M. Masson,et al. Repetition blindness in rapid lists: activation and inhibition versus construction and attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[20] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual object processing in optic aphasia: a case of semantic access agnosia , 1987 .
[21] Christopher Barry,et al. Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement , 1997 .
[22] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Concepts and categories: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.
[23] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[24] K. Heilman,et al. Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use , 1989, Annals of neurology.
[25] N. Kanwisher. Repetition blindness: Type recognition without token individuation , 1987, Cognition.
[26] F. Fang,et al. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[28] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[29] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Action-Related Properties Shape Object Representations in the Ventral Stream , 2007, Neuron.
[30] James L. McClelland,et al. Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. , 2004, Psychological review.
[31] M. Bar,et al. Cortical Mechanisms Specific to Explicit Visual Object Recognition , 2001, Neuron.
[32] M Poncet,et al. The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. A case of multimodal agnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[33] Roel M. Willems,et al. The Function of Words: Distinct Neural Correlates for Words Denoting Differently Manipulable Objects , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] D J Mewhort,et al. Repetition deficit in rapid-serial-visual-presentation displays: encoding failure or retrieval failure? , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Mary L Still,et al. Now you see it, now you don't: repetition blindness for nonwords. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[36] M. Aravindh. Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2010 .
[37] P. Jolicoeur. The time to name disoriented natural objects , 1985, Memory & cognition.
[38] M. Corballis,et al. Repetition blindness is orientation blind , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[39] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[40] R. Malach,et al. Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[41] Nicolas A. McNair,et al. Disentangling the contributions of grasp and action representations in the recognition of manipulable objects , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[42] Veronika Coltheart,et al. Repetition blindness for novel objects , 2005 .
[43] Paul E. Dux,et al. Orientation-invariant object recognition: evidence from repetition blindness , 2005, Cognition.
[44] A. Caramazza,et al. The locus of the frequency effect in picture naming: When recognizing is not enough , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[45] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. The Role of the Dorsal Visual Processing Stream in Tool Identification , 2010, Psychological science.
[46] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[47] 柳 悦州,et al. Structure and deterioration of silk fabrics of the edo period. , 2000 .
[48] A. Sirigu,et al. The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. A case of multimodal agnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[49] Irina M Harris,et al. Turning objects on their heads: The influence of the stored axis on object individuation , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] K. Grill-Spector,et al. The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[51] S. Andrews,et al. Repetition blindness in sentence contexts: Not just an attribution? , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[52] Glyn W Humphreys,et al. Privileged access to action for objects relative to words , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[53] A Caramazza,et al. Repetition blindness under minimum memory load: Effects of spatial and temporal proximity and the encoding effectiveness of the first item , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[54] A. Morris,et al. Repetition blindness: An emergent property of inter-item competition , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[55] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[56] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. Category-specific neural processing for naming pictures of animals and naming pictures of tools: An ALE meta-analysis , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[57] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Brain Signatures of Meaning Access in Action Word Recognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[58] J. Haxby,et al. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[59] Markus Graf,et al. The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[60] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Neural events and perceptual awareness , 2001, Cognition.
[61] G. Humphreys,et al. Modelling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM) , 2002 .
[62] M. Potter,et al. Repetition blindness: levels of processing. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] F. Fang,et al. Cortical responses to invisible objects in human dorsal and ventral pathways , 2010 .
[64] W. Glaser. Picture naming , 1992, Cognition.
[65] M. Gardner,et al. USING CONFIDENCE INTERVALS , 1987, The Lancet.
[66] Alice Mado Proverbio,et al. 250ms to code for action affordance during observation of manipulable objects , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[67] O. Hauk,et al. Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto‐central cortex , 2004, Human brain mapping.