The mental representations of fractions: adults' same–different judgments
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Automaticity in Processing Ordinal Information , 2004 .
[2] S Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[4] Avishai Henik,et al. SUPPRESSION SITUATIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH : DEFINITIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND APPLICATIONS , 1991 .
[5] Avishai Henik,et al. SUPPRESSION SITUATIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH : DEFINITIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND APPLICATIONS , 1991 .
[6] Thomas J. Faulkenberry,et al. Mental representations in fraction comparison. , 2011, Experimental psychology.
[7] V. Csépe,et al. The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[8] Marco Zorzi,et al. The mental representation of numerical fractions: real or integer? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[10] Margarete Delazer,et al. The processing and representation of fractions within the brain An fMRI investigation , 2009, NeuroImage.
[11] Arava Y. Kallai,et al. A generalized fraction: an entity smaller than one on the mental number line. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] Michael Schneider,et al. Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] Dale J Cohen,et al. Integers do not automatically activate their quantity representation , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] H. Bergh,et al. Examples of Mixed-Effects Modeling with Crossed Random Effects and with Binomial Data. , 2008 .
[15] Xinlin Zhou,et al. Holistic or compositional representation of two-digit numbers? Evidence from the distance, magnitude, and SNARC effects in a number-matching task , 2008, Cognition.
[16] Andreas Nieder,et al. Notation-Independent Representation of Fractions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[18] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Comparing 5/7 and 2/9: Adults can do it by accessing the magnitude of the whole fractions. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[19] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[20] C. Gallistel,et al. The Cultural and Evolutionary History of the Real Numbers , 2001 .
[21] Andreas Nieder,et al. Behavioral and Prefrontal Representation of Spatial Proportions in the Monkey , 2008, Current Biology.
[22] Nancy K. Mack. Learning rational numbers with understanding: The case of informal knowledge. , 1993 .
[23] Koleen McCrink,et al. Ratio Abstraction by 6-Month-Old Infants , 2007, Psychological science.
[24] T. Verguts,et al. The origins of the numerical distance effect: The same–different task , 2011 .