Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Torsten Schubert,et al. Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] R. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] Jeff Miller,et al. Backward response-level crosstalk in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] D. V. von Cramon,et al. Localization of Executive Functions in Dual-Task Performance with fMRI , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[5] S. Chaiken,et al. Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .
[6] H. Pashler,et al. Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] R W Proctor,et al. Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[9] M. Ferguson,et al. How social perception can automatically influence behavior , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[10] D E Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.
[11] John R. Anderson,et al. Serial modules in parallel: the psychological refractory period and perfect time-sharing. , 2001, Psychological review.
[12] Morris Moscovitch,et al. A Neuropsychological Model of Memory and Consciousness , 2008 .
[13] J. C. Johnston,et al. Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference , 1992 .
[14] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Simultaneous cognitive operations in working memory after dual-task practice. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] G W Humphreys,et al. The combined effects of plane disorientation and foreshortening on picture naming: one manipulation or two? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Richard T. Kelly,et al. Secondary task performance during directed forgetting , 1974 .
[17] F. Craik,et al. The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[18] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] H. Pashler. The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .
[20] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] L. Jacoby. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .
[22] Alexander Todorov,et al. Spontaneous trait inferences are bound to actors' faces: evidence from a false recognition paradigm. , 2002 .
[23] G D Logan,et al. Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] D. Rohrer. On the relative and absolute strength of a memory trace , 1996, Memory & cognition.
[25] Alexander Todorov,et al. The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors’ faces , 2003 .
[26] Jennifer M. Glass,et al. Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.
[27] J. Duncan. Divided attention: the whole is more than the sum of its parts. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] M. Posner,et al. Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. , 1976, Psychological review.
[29] Pierre Jolicœur,et al. All-or-none bottleneck versus capacity sharing accounts of the psychological refractory period phenomenon , 2002, Psychological research.
[30] A. D. Smith,et al. Effects of age and a divided attention task presented during encoding and retrieval on memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[31] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] Torsten Schubert,et al. Aging and input processing in dual-task situations. , 2004, Psychology and aging.
[34] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Symbolic distance affects two processing loci in the number comparison task , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[35] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[36] G D Logan,et al. Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: II. Episodic memory. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[37] D. Navon,et al. Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] Jeff Miller,et al. Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies , 2006, Psychological research.
[39] H Pashler,et al. Attentional limits in memory retrieval. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[40] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.
[41] Torsten Schubert,et al. Valence processing bypassing the response selection bottleneck? Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2008, Experimental psychology.
[42] J. Danckert. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[43] J. Uleman,et al. Spontaneous versus intentional inferences in impression formation , 1999 .
[44] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[45] M. Brysbaert. Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .
[46] Stefanie Schuch,et al. The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[47] S. Dehaene,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] Ann Reynolds,et al. The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] M. Ferguson,et al. Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.
[50] Larry R. Squire,et al. Neuropsychology of memory, 2nd ed. , 1992 .
[51] Torsten Schubert,et al. Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks , 1999 .
[52] Welford At. An apparatus for use in studying serial performance. , 1952 .
[53] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[54] H. Pashler,et al. Is dual-task slowing instruction dependent? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] A. Baddeley,et al. Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. , 1984 .
[56] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[57] John R. Anderson,et al. Learning to achieve perfect timesharing: architectural implications of Hazeltine, Teague, and Ivry (2002). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[58] J. Wixted,et al. An analysis of latency and interresponse time in free recall , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[59] M Naveh-Benjamin,et al. The Effects of Divided Attention on Encoding and Retrieval Processes: The Resiliency of Retrieval Processes , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[60] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[61] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Testing the predictions of the central capacity sharing model. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] T. Woodward,et al. Bivalency is costly: bivalent stimuli elicit cautious responding. , 2003, Experimental psychology.
[63] S Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[64] Roy Luria,et al. Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[65] David E. Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .
[66] D. Navon,et al. Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[67] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] Roman Liepelt. Learning Mechanisms Enabling Perfect Time-sharing in Dual Tasks , 2006 .