'Thermal taste' predicts higher responsiveness to chemical taste and flavor.

Individual differences in taste perception have been explained in part by variations in peripheral innervation associated with the genetic ability to taste the bitter substances PTC and PROP. In the present study we report evidence of another source of individual differences that is independent of taste stimulus, taste quality, or gustatory nerve. Individuals who perceived taste from thermal stimulation alone (thermal taste) gave significantly higher taste ratings to chemical stimuli--often by a factor of >2:1--than did individuals who perceived no taste from thermal stimulation. This was true for all taste stimuli tested (sucrose, saccharin, sodium chloride, citric acid, quinine sulfate, MSG and PROP), for all three gustatory areas of the mouth (anterior tongue, posterior tongue and soft palate) and for whole-mouth stimulation. Moreover, the same individuals reported stronger sensations from the olfactory stimulus vanillin, particularly when it was sensed retronasally. The generality of the thermal-taster advantage and its extension to an olfactory stimulus suggests that it arises from individual differences in CNS processes that are involved in perception of both taste and flavor.

[1]  B. Green,et al.  The effect of cooling on the perception of carbohydrate and intensive sweeteners , 1988, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  H. Rollin,et al.  Course of the Peripheral Gustatory Nerves , 1977, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[3]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Individual differences in sensitivity to bitter-tasting substances , 1993 .

[4]  J. C. Stevens,et al.  PTC taste blindness and the taste of caffeine , 1975, Nature.

[5]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Both perceptual and conceptual factors influence taste-odor and taste-taste interactions , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  W S Cain,et al.  Mutual action of taste and olfaction. , 1977, Sensory processes.

[7]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Dissociation of Neural Representation of Intensity and Affective Valuation in Human Gustation , 2003, Neuron.

[8]  L. Bartoshuk The biological basis of food perception and acceptance , 1993 .

[9]  B Cowart,et al.  Evaluating the 'Labeled Magnitude Scale' for measuring sensations of taste and smell. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[10]  A. Drewnowski,et al.  Psychophysical Measurement of 6‐n‐Propylthiouracil (PROP) Taste Perception a , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  H. Lawless,et al.  Cross adaptation of sucrose and intensive sweeteners , 1983 .

[12]  H. Stone,et al.  Temperature and ph effects on the relative sweetness of suprathreshold mixtures of dextrose fructose , 1969 .

[13]  H. Lawless,et al.  Bitter taste of saccharin and acesulfame-K. , 2002, Chemical senses.

[14]  N. Ryba,et al.  The Receptors for Mammalian Sweet and Umami Taste , 2003, Cell.

[15]  M. Posner,et al.  Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  P. Breslin,et al.  Relationship of papillae number to bitter intensity of quinine and PROP within and between individuals , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.

[17]  Jayaram Chandrashekar,et al.  A Novel Family of Mammalian Taste Receptors , 2000, Cell.

[18]  M. Inoue,et al.  Allelic Variation of the Tas1r3 Taste Receptor Gene Selectively Affects Behavioral and Neural Taste Responses to Sweeteners in the F2 Hybrids between C57BL/6ByJ and 129P3/J Mice , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  B. Green,et al.  Thermal stimulation of taste , 2000, Nature.

[20]  B. Green,et al.  The effect of cooling the tongue on the perceived intensity of taste , 1987 .

[21]  I. Wakeling,et al.  Binary taste mixture interactions in prop non-tasters, medium-tasters and super-tasters. , 2001, Chemical senses.

[22]  Alain Dagher,et al.  Feeding-induced dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasantness ratings in healthy human volunteers , 2003, NeuroImage.

[23]  R. Fischer,et al.  Quinine Dimorphism: A Cardinal Determinant of Taste Sensitivity , 1963, Nature.

[24]  A. Calviño Perception of sweetness: The effects of concentration and temperature , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[25]  H. Schifferstein,et al.  The perception of the taste of KCl, NaCl and quinineHCl is not related to PROP-sensitivity , 1991 .

[26]  Mark Leppert,et al.  Positional Cloning of the Human Quantitative Trait Locus Underlying Taste Sensitivity to Phenylthiocarbamide , 2003, Science.

[27]  P. Rozin,et al.  “Taste-smell confusions” and the duality of the olfactory sense , 1982, Perception & Psychophysics.

[28]  B. Green,et al.  "Warmth-insensitive fields": evidence of sparse and irregular innervation of human skin by the warmth sense. , 1998, Somatosensory & motor research.

[29]  K. Paulus,et al.  The influence of temperature on the threshold values of primary tastes , 1980 .

[30]  Y. Yasoshima,et al.  Roles of chemical mediators in the taste system. , 1998, Japanese journal of pharmacology.

[31]  R. Boakes,et al.  Confusing tastes and smells: how odours can influence the perception of sweet and sour tastes. , 1999, Chemical senses.

[32]  U. Hegerl,et al.  Sensory cortical processing and the biological basis of personality , 1995, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  R. Lindman,et al.  Interindividual Differences in Scaling Performance , 1968, Perceptual and motor skills.

[34]  Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al.  PTC/PROP tasting: Anatomy, psychophysics, and sex effects , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[35]  H. Kalmus,et al.  THE MEASUREMENT OF TASTE SENSITIVITY TO PHENYLTHIOUREA (P.T.C.) , 1949 .

[36]  Yngve Zotterman,et al.  Action potentials in the glossopharyngeal nerve and in the chorda tympani1 , 1935 .

[37]  L. Bartoshuk,et al.  Comparing sensory experiences across individuals: recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic variation in taste perception. , 2000, Chemical senses.

[38]  B. Tepper,et al.  PROP Taster Status Is Related to Fat Perception and Preference a , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[39]  B. Green,et al.  Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties , 1993 .

[40]  R. Bradley,et al.  Demonstration of sensory innervation of rat tongue with anterogradely transported horseradish peroxidase , 1986, Brain Research.

[41]  I. J. Miller,et al.  Variations in human taste bud density and taste intensity perception , 1990, Physiology & Behavior.

[42]  Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al.  Labeled scales (e.g., category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: what we have learned from genetic variation in taste , 2003 .

[43]  E. Rolls,et al.  Taste‐olfactory convergence, and the representation of the pleasantness of flavour, in the human brain , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  Georg Juckel,et al.  Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission: A new hypothesis , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[45]  Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al.  Sweetness of sucrose, neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, and saccharin is related to genetic ability to taste the bitter substance 6-n-propylthiouracil , 1983 .

[46]  S. Harada,et al.  Different characteristics of gustatory responses between the greater superficial petrosal and chorda tympani nerves in the rat. , 1997, Chemical senses.

[47]  Lawrence E. Marks,et al.  Bitterness of KCl and benzoate: Related to genetic status for sensitivity to PTC/PROP. , 1988 .

[48]  D. Mcburney Gustatory cross adaptation between sweet-tasting compounds , 1972 .

[49]  V. Duffy,et al.  Supertasting, earaches and head injury: Genetics and pathology alter our taste worlds , 1996, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[50]  H. Weinstein,et al.  Tas1r3, encoding a new candidate taste receptor, is allelic to the sweet responsiveness locus Sac , 2001, Nature Genetics.

[51]  V. Duffy,et al.  PROP (6‐n‐Propylthiouracil) Supertasters and the Saltiness of NaCl a , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[52]  J. Rodin,et al.  Effects of temperature on the perceived sweetness of sucrose , 1982, Physiology & Behavior.

[53]  R. Doty,et al.  NaCl thresholds: relationship to anterior tongue locus, area of stimulation, and number of fungiform papillae , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.

[54]  G. Ekman,et al.  Saltness, sweetness, and preference. A study of quantitative relations in individual subjects. , 1965, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[55]  Robert A. Frank,et al.  Taste–smell interactions are tastant and odorant dependent , 1988 .

[56]  H. Kalmus,et al.  Chemical specificity in genetical differences of taste sensitivity. , 1949, Annals of eugenics.