Reasoning about fraction and decimal magnitudes, reasoning proportionally, and mathematics achievement in Australia and the United States

There is strong evidence from research conducted in the United States that fraction magnitude understanding supports mathematics achievement. Unfortunately, there has been little research that examines if this relation is present across educational contexts with different approaches to teaching fractions. The current study compared fourth and sixth grade students from two countries which differ in their approach to teaching fractions: Australia and the United States. We gathered data on fraction and decimal magnitude understanding, proportional reasoning, and a standardized mathematics achievement test on whole number computation. Across both countries, reasoning about rational magnitude (either fraction or decimal) was predictive of whole number computation, supporting the central role of rational number learning. However, the precise relation varied, indicating that cross-national differences in rational number instruction can influence the nature of the relation between understanding fraction and decimal magnitude and mathematics achievement. The relation between proportional reasoning and whole number computation was fully mediated by rational magnitude understanding, suggesting that a key mechanism for how reasoning about rational magnitude supports mathematics achievement: proportional reasoning supports the development of an accurate spatial representation of magnitude that can be flexibly and proportionally scaled, which in turn supports children’s mathematics learning. Together, these findings support using measurement models and spatial scaling strategies when teaching fractions and decimals.

[1]  Elizabeth A. Gunderson,et al.  Relations among spatial skills, number line estimation, and exact and approximate calculation in young children. , 2021, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  Xiao Zhang,et al.  Visual-spatial skills contribute to Chinese reading and arithmetic for different reasons: A three-wave longitudinal study. , 2021, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  Julie Lorah Interpretation of main effects in the presence of non-significant interaction effects , 2020 .

[4]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  How many apples make a quarter? The challenge of discrete proportional formats. , 2020, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  PISA 2018 Results (Volume I) , 2019, PISA.

[6]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Improving fraction understanding in sixth graders with mathematics difficulties: Effects of a number line approach combined with cognitive learning strategies. , 2019, Journal of Educational Psychology.

[7]  D. Ansari,et al.  Relations between numerical, spatial, and executive function skills and mathematics achievement: A latent-variable approach , 2019, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza,et al.  Psychometric Properties of the WRAT Math Computation Subtest in Mexican Adolescents , 2018, Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment.

[9]  Luke F. Rinne,et al.  Children’s Reasoning About Decimals and Its Relation to Fraction Learning and Mathematics Achievement , 2018, Journal of Educational Psychology.

[10]  Yingyi Liu Fraction magnitude understanding and its unique role in predicting general mathematics achievement at two early stages of fraction instruction , 2018, The British journal of educational psychology.

[11]  Luke F. Rinne,et al.  Co-development of fraction magnitude knowledge and mathematics achievement from fourth through sixth grade , 2017 .

[12]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Making Space for Spatial Proportions , 2017, Journal of learning disabilities.

[13]  L. Doyle,et al.  Changing Neurodevelopment at 8 Years in Children Born Extremely Preterm Since the 1990s , 2017, Pediatrics.

[14]  Stephen D. Short,et al.  Determining Power and Sample Size for Simple and Complex Mediation Models , 2017 .

[15]  Elizabeth A. Gunderson,et al.  The Number Line Is a Critical Spatial-Numerical Representation: Evidence From a Fraction Intervention , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[16]  Luke F. Rinne,et al.  Development of Fraction Comparison Strategies: A Latent Transition Analysis , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[17]  Luke F. Rinne,et al.  Pathways to fraction learning: Numerical abilities mediate the relation between early cognitive competencies and later fraction knowledge. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  Ilyse Resnick,et al.  Developmental growth trajectories in understanding of fraction magnitude from fourth through sixth grade. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[19]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders’ knowledge of fractions ☆ , 2015 .

[20]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Bridging the Gap: Fraction Understanding Is Central to Mathematics Achievement in Students from Three Different Continents. , 2015 .

[21]  Ty W. Boyer,et al.  Prompting children to reason proportionally: Processing discrete units as continuous amounts. , 2015, Developmental psychology.

[22]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Conceptual structure and the procedural affordances of rational numbers: relational reasoning with fractions and decimals. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  An Integrative Theory of Numerical Development. , 2014 .

[24]  Jennifer M. Tobias,et al.  Mathematical Content Knowledge for Teaching Elementary Mathematics: A Focus on Fractions , 2014, The Mathematics Enthusiast.

[25]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  The impact of fraction magnitude knowledge on algebra performance and learning. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[26]  Aryn A Pyke,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in understanding of fractions. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[27]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Improving At-Risk Learners' Understanding of Fractions , 2013 .

[28]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Competence with fractions predicts gains in mathematics achievement. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Fractions: Could they really be the gatekeeper’s doorman? , 2012 .

[30]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Getting the big picture: Development of spatial scaling abilities , 2012 .

[31]  R. Siegler,et al.  Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.

[32]  Glenn Rowley,et al.  Knowledge of future primary teachers for teaching mathematics: an international comparative study , 2012 .

[33]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[34]  Ty W. Boyer,et al.  Child proportional scaling: is 1/3=2/6=3/9=4/12? , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  A. Hayes Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical Mediation Analysis in the New Millennium , 2009 .

[38]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[39]  Elida V. Laski,et al.  Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.

[40]  Matthew S. Fritz,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Required Sample Size to Detect the Mediated Effect , 2022 .

[41]  Leif Engqvist,et al.  The mistreatment of covariate interaction terms in linear model analyses of behavioural and evolutionary ecology studies , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Spatial Scaling in Young Children , 1999 .

[43]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Spatial scaling, proportional thinking, and numerical understanding in 5- to 7-year-old children , 2018 .

[44]  L. Thompson,et al.  Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability , 2007 .