Visual Selection and the Human Frontal Eye Fields: Effects of Frontal Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on Partial Report Analyzed by Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention

While the frontal eye fields (FEF) are traditionally associated with eye movements, recent work indicates possible roles in controlling selective visual processing. We applied 10 Hz bursts of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over left or right human FEF while subjects performed a partial-report task that allowed quantitative estimates of top-down control and other parameters affecting visual performance. Participants selectively reported digits in a relevant color (targets) but not those in an irrelevant color (nontargets) from a brief masked display. A target could appear alone or together with an accompanying item (nontarget or target) in the same or opposite hemifield. Targets were normally identified better when presented with a nontarget than with another target, indicating prioritization of task-relevant targets and thus top-down control. We found this usual pattern of results without TMS, and also with TMS over left FEF. However, during right FEF TMS, the detrimental impact of accompanying distractors increased. Formal analysis in terms of Bundesen's (1990) theory of visual attention confirmed that right FEF TMS diminished the top-down control parameter for both hemifields, indicating an FEF role in top-down selection even for targets defined by the nonspatial property of color. Direct comparison with our previous findings for parietal TMS (Hung et al., 2005) confirmed the distinct role of FEF in top-down control, plus right-hemisphere predominance for this in humans.

[1]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  FEF TMS affects visual cortical activity. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  T. Moore,et al.  Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Concurrent TMS-fMRI and Psychophysics Reveal Frontal Influences on Human Retinotopic Visual Cortex , 2006, Current Biology.

[4]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left human frontal eye fields eliminates the cost of invalid endogenous cues , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  G Humphreys,et al.  Systematic analysis of deficits in visual attention. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Left frontal eye field remembers “where” but not “what” , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[12]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[13]  J. Tanji,et al.  Oculomotor sequence learning: a positron emission tomography study , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  T. Cornsweet,et al.  The staircrase-method in psychophysics. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[15]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search , 1993, Nature.

[18]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention and short-term memory (NTVA) , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  E. J. Tehovnik,et al.  Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates , 2000, Brain Research Reviews.

[21]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Jon Driver,et al.  Visual Selection and Posterior Parietal Cortex: Effects of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation on Partial Report Analyzed by Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Tomás Paus,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Human Frontal Eye ®eld Facilitates Visual Awareness , 2022 .

[24]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.

[25]  H. Kennedy,et al.  Laminar Distribution of Neurons in Extrastriate Areas Projecting to Visual Areas V1 and V4 Correlates with the Hierarchical Rank and Indicates the Operation of a Distance Rule , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J D Schall,et al.  Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Topographic organization of the middle temporal visual area in the macaque monkey: Representational biases and the relationship to callosal connections and myeloarchitectonic boundaries , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Selective Attention from Voluntary Control of Neurons in Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, Science.

[30]  John Duncan,et al.  Attentional functions of parietal and frontal cortex. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  T. Paus,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Human Frontal Eye Field: Effects on Visual Perception and Attention , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  Timing of Target Discrimination in Human Frontal Eye Fields , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2003, Nature.

[35]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  A. Cowey,et al.  Visual field defects after frontal eye-field lesions in monkeys. , 1971, Brain research.

[37]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Neural correlates of visual and motor decision processes , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[39]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Frontal eye fields involved in shifting frame of reference within working memory for scenes , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  O. Blanke,et al.  Location of the human frontal eye field as defined by electrical cortical stimulation: anatomical, functional and electrophysiological characteristics , 2000, Neuroreport.

[41]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  Stimulation of the human frontal eye fields modulates sensitivity of extrastriate visual cortex. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  J. Driver,et al.  Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  Claus Bundesen,et al.  Patient assessment based on a theory of visual attention (TVA): subtle deficits after a right frontal-subcortical lesion , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Chi-Hung Juan,et al.  Human frontal eye fields and visual search. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  T. Paus Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: A selective review , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Feature-Based Attention in the Frontal Eye Field and Area V4 during Visual Search , 2011, Neuron.

[48]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Hemispheric Differences in Frontal and Parietal Influences on Human Occipital Cortex: Direct Confirmation with Concurrent TMS–fMRI , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.