A normal intensity level of psycho-physiological stress can benefit working memory performance at high load

Abstract In recent years, the effects of stress on adaptation and human performance have received considerable attention in medical literature and in occupational fields. In this study, we employed an acute psycho-physiological laboratory stressor, which is similar to real life stressful conditions and is capable of reliably eliciting the normal range of stress effects. The primary goal of the present study was to investigate whether the effect of stress on working memory performance depends on the number of images that need to be maintained (memory load), and the duration of the retention interval. By manipulating the number of images and the length of the retention interval of the modified Stenberg working memory test, we altered the task difficulty and examined the consequent effects on behavior. Twelve young students were submitted to a stressful condition one day and a control condition on another day. The stressor used included a physiological factor (socially evaluated cold pressor test) and a psychosocial factor (modified version of trier social stress test). The sympathetic nervous system and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activation were assessed by heart rate and salivary cortisol. It was found that this procedure, which involves exposure to a normal intensity level of psycho-physiological stress, can demonstrate the beneficial effect of stress on reaction time without decreasing accuracy at high memory loads, but not at low memory loads. Relevance to industry Managing stress in the workplace is important for decreasing human errors. Workers' performances can be improved if they receive appropriate levels of stress and task difficulty. If so, they might gain an advantage as long-term memory can be supported in addition to working memory.

[1]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Frequency characteristics of evoked and oscillatory electroencephalic activity in a human memory scanning task , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[2]  S. Sternberg High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 1966, Science.

[3]  Brian Peacock,et al.  A fuzzy sets modelling approach for ergonomic workload stress analysis , 1994 .

[4]  Kilseop Ryu,et al.  Evaluation of mental workload with a combined measure based on physiological indices during a dual task of tracking and mental arithmetic , 2005 .

[5]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  The role of the hippocampus in retaining relational information across short delays: the importance of memory load. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[6]  O. Wolf,et al.  Impaired Memory Retrieval after Psychosocial Stress in Healthy Young Men , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  L. Squire,et al.  Working memory, long-term memory, and medial temporal lobe function. , 2011, Learning & memory.

[8]  Christian Grillon,et al.  Working memory performance after acute exposure to the cold pressor stress in healthy volunteers , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[9]  Ming Zhang,et al.  Spectral analysis of cerebral oxygenation responses to seated whole-body vibration in healthy men , 2012 .

[10]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[11]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[12]  N. Ellis,et al.  Working Memory in the Acquisition of Vocabulary and Syntax: Putting Language in Good Order , 1996 .

[13]  C. Grillon,et al.  Acute exposure to stress improves performance in trace eyeblink conditioning and spatial learning tasks in healthy men. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[14]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  Joseph M. Andreano,et al.  Glucocorticoid Release and Memory Consolidation in Men and Women , 2006, Psychological science.

[16]  S. Luck,et al.  Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory , 2008, Nature.

[17]  Thomas Rupp,et al.  NIRS in ergonomics: Its application in industry for promotion of health and human performance at work , 2010 .

[18]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[19]  B. Roozendaal Systems mediating acute glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation and retrieval , 2003, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  Pranab Kumar Nag,et al.  Do the work stress factors of women telephone operators change with the shift schedules , 2004 .

[21]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  Working Memory and the Organization of Brain Systems , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  The 'Trier Social Stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[23]  Michael E. Smith,et al.  Monitoring Working Memory Load during Computer-Based Tasks with EEG Pattern Recognition Methods , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[24]  Anjan Chatterjee,et al.  Visual Working Memory Is Impaired when the Medial Temporal Lobe Is Damaged , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[26]  Oliver T. Wolf,et al.  Stress and memory in humans: Twelve years of progress? , 2009, Brain Research.

[27]  Alberto Fernández,et al.  Activity in human medial temporal lobe associated with encoding process in spatial working memory revealed by magnetoencephalography , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  B. Bermond,et al.  Psychosocial stress impairs working memory at high loads: An association with cortisol levels and memory retrieval , 2006, Stress.

[29]  Carmen Sandi,et al.  A facilitative role for corticosterone in the acquisition of a spatial task under moderate stress. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[30]  Theo G. M. van Erp,et al.  Hippocampal activations during encoding and retrieval in a verbal working memory paradigm , 2005, NeuroImage.

[31]  D. Drachman,et al.  Memory and the hippocampal complex. II. Is memory a multiple process , 1966 .

[32]  J. Palva,et al.  New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  L. Schwabe,et al.  HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test , 2008, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[34]  Martin Lepage,et al.  Stress, memory, and the hippocampus: can't live with it, can't live without it , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[35]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[36]  E. Vogel,et al.  Discrete capacity limits in visual working memory , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  P. Hancock,et al.  An investigation of heat stress effects on time-sharing performance , 2002, Ergonomics.

[38]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[39]  David M. Diamond,et al.  The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.