Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy.

The ability to quickly detect potential threat is an important survival mechanism for humans and other animals. Past research has established that adults have an attentional bias for the detection of threat-relevant stimuli, including snakes and spiders as well as angry human faces. Recent studies have documented that preschool children also detect the presence of threatening stimuli more quickly than various non-threatening stimuli. Here we report the first evidence that this attentional bias is present even in infancy. In two experiments, 8- to 14-month-old infants responded more rapidly to snakes than to flowers and more rapidly to angry than to happy faces. These data provide the first evidence of enhanced visual detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infants and hence offer especially strong support for the existence of a general bias for the detection of threat in humans.

[1]  I. Blanchette Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: How specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  O. Lipp,et al.  Snakes and cats in the flower bed: fast detection is not specific to pictures of fear-relevant animals. , 2004, Emotion.

[3]  Vanessa Lobue,et al.  More than just another face in the crowd: superior detection of threatening facial expressions in children and adults. , 2009, Developmental science.

[4]  O. Lipp,et al.  Attentional bias toward fear-related stimuli: an investigation with nonselected children and adults and children with anxiety disorders. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition , 1993 .

[6]  T. Striano,et al.  Neural processing of eye gaze and threat-related emotional facial expressions in infancy. , 2008, Child development.

[7]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Categorical Perception of Facial Expressions by 7-Month-Old Infants , 2001, Perception.

[8]  G. Gendolla,et al.  Detecting emotional faces and features in a visual search paradigm: are faces special? , 2006, Emotion.

[9]  S. Mineka,et al.  Second-order conditioning and overshadowing in the observational conditioning of fear in monkeys. , 1987, Behaviour research and therapy.

[10]  J. Deloache,et al.  Detecting the Snake in the Grass , 2008, Psychological science.

[11]  F. Esteves Attentional bias to emotional facial expressions , 1999 .

[12]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[13]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The effects of movement of internal features on infants' preferences for face-like stimuli , 1992 .

[15]  T. Striano,et al.  The discrimination of angry and fearful facial expressions in 7-month-old infants: An event-related potential study , 2008 .

[16]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Daniel Lundqvist,et al.  Emotion regulates attention: The relation between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual attention , 2005 .

[18]  A. Flykt,et al.  Visual search with biological threat stimuli: accuracy, reaction times, and heart rate changes. , 2005, Emotion.

[19]  S. Mineka,et al.  Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. , 1989, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  CONSPEC and CONLERN: a two-process theory of infant face recognition. , 1991, Psychological review.

[21]  J. Krystal,et al.  Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the Archives of General Psychiatry. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[22]  Daniel Lundqvist,et al.  Facilitated detection of angry faces: Initial orienting and processing efficiency , 2006 .

[23]  J. Deloache,et al.  The narrow fellow in the grass: human infants associate snakes and fear. , 2009, Developmental science.

[24]  Leslie B. Cohen,et al.  Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. , 1972, Child development.

[25]  R. Kleck,et al.  Perceived Gaze Direction and the Processing of Facial Displays of Emotion , 2003, Psychological science.

[26]  E. Fox,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.

[27]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  T. Brosch,et al.  The role of fear-relevant stimuli in visual search: a comparison of phylogenetic and ontogenetic stimuli. , 2005, Emotion.

[29]  S. Rauch,et al.  A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. , 2001, Emotion.

[30]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in Asperger Syndrome , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  J. Werker,et al.  Matching phonetic information in lips and voice is robust in 4.5-month-old infants , 1999 .

[32]  P. Green Biology and Cognitive Development: the Case of Face Recognition, Mark H. Johnson, John Morton. Blackwell, Oxford (1991), x, +180. Price £35.00 hardback, £10.95 paperback , 1992 .

[33]  Daniel Mills,et al.  Left gaze bias in humans, rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[34]  D. Rakison,et al.  Do infants possess an evolved spider-detection mechanism? , 2008, Cognition.

[35]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[36]  Andrew W Young,et al.  The eyebrow frown: a salient social signal. , 2002, Emotion.

[37]  L. Isbell,et al.  Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. , 2006, Journal of human evolution.

[38]  M. Seligman On the generality of the laws of learning , 1970 .

[39]  Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. , 1972, Child development.

[40]  M Studdert-Kennedy,et al.  Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. , 1983, Science.

[41]  A. Walker,et al.  Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[42]  D. Heitjan,et al.  Mixed-effects models in psychophysiology. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Arne Öhman,et al.  Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms. , 1993 .

[44]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[45]  M. Mather,et al.  Angry faces get noticed quickly: threat detection is not impaired among older adults. , 2006, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.