Spatiotemporal oscillatory dynamics of visual selective attention during a flanker task

&NA; The flanker task is a test of visual selective attention that has been widely used to probe error monitoring, response conflict, and related constructs. However, to date, few studies have focused on the selective attention component of this task and imaged the underlying oscillatory dynamics serving task performance. In this study, 21 healthy adults successfully completed an arrow‐based version of the Eriksen flanker task during magnetoencephalography (MEG). All MEG data were pre‐processed and transformed into the time‐frequency domain. Significant oscillatory brain responses were imaged using a beamforming approach, and voxel time series were extracted from the peak responses to identify the temporal dynamics. Across both congruent and incongruent flanker conditions, our results indicated robust decreases in alpha (9–12 Hz) activity in medial and lateral occipital regions, bilateral parietal cortices, and cerebellar areas during task performance. In parallel, increases in theta (3–7 Hz) oscillatory activity were detected in dorsal and ventral frontal regions, and the anterior cingulate. As per conditional effects, stronger alpha responses (i.e., greater desynchronization) were observed in parietal, occipital, and cerebellar cortices during incongruent relative to congruent trials, whereas the opposite pattern emerged for theta responses (i.e., synchronization) in the anterior cingulate, left dorsolateral prefrontal, and ventral prefrontal cortices. Interestingly, the peak latency of theta responses in these latter brain regions was significantly correlated with reaction time, and may partially explain the amplitude difference observed between congruent and incongruent trials. Lastly, whole‐brain exploratory analyses implicated the frontal eye fields, right temporoparietal junction, and premotor cortices. These findings suggest that regions of both the dorsal and ventral attention networks contribute to visual selective attention processes during incongruent trials, and that such differential processes are transient and fully completed shortly after the behavioral response in most trials. HighlightsHealthy adults performed a visual selective attention task while undergoing MEG.Participants exhibited significant theta and alpha oscillations during the task.These responses were imaged with a beamformer and voxel time series were extracted.Results showed occipital and parietal alpha activity and theta in prefrontal cortex.Oscillatory dynamics within attention networks distinguished attention conditions.

[1]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[3]  S. Taulu,et al.  Applications of the signal space separation method , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Signal Processing.

[4]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[5]  M. D. Ernst Permutation Methods: A Basis for Exact Inference , 2004 .

[6]  L. P. Zapata,et al.  Exogenous capture of medial–frontal oscillatory mechanisms by unattended conflicting information , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[8]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Hidenao Fukuyama,et al.  Pre-SMA actively engages in conflict processing in human: A combined study of epicortical ERPs and direct cortical stimulation , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Deconstructing the Architecture of Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems with Dynamic Causal Modeling , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Tony W Wilson,et al.  Coding complexity in the human motor circuit , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[12]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[13]  Krish D. Singh,et al.  A new approach to neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[15]  Avinash R. Vaidya,et al.  Evidence for a Role for the Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Disengaging from an Incorrect Action , 2014, PLoS ONE.

[16]  G. Fink,et al.  Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems : Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles , 2013 .

[17]  Sam M. Doesburg,et al.  Top-down alpha oscillatory network interactions during visuospatial attention orienting , 2016, NeuroImage.

[18]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  J Driver,et al.  A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Mapping the functional anatomy of task preparation: Priming task‐appropriate brain networks , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[23]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling , 2014, Trends in Neurosciences.

[24]  D. Norman Toward a theory of memory and attention. , 1968 .

[25]  Jan R. Wessel,et al.  Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[26]  Peter E. Clayson,et al.  Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects: Support for the conflict monitoring theory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  J Gross,et al.  REPRINTS , 1962, The Lancet.

[28]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  S. Wiens,et al.  Behavioral and ERP indices of response conflict in Stroop and flanker tasks. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[30]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[31]  S. Taulu,et al.  Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements , 2006, Physics in medicine and biology.

[32]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[34]  Alex I. Wiesman,et al.  Quiet connections: Reduced fronto‐temporal connectivity in nondemented Parkinson's Disease during working memory encoding , 2016, Human Brain Mapping.

[35]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Subthreshold muscle twitches dissociate oscillatory neural signatures of conflicts from errors , 2014, NeuroImage.

[36]  James F. Cavanagh,et al.  Multiple aspects of the stress response under social evaluative threat: An electrophysiological investigation , 2008, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[37]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Single-Trial Regression Elucidates the Role of Prefrontal Theta Oscillations in Response Conflict , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[38]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Signal-space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components , 1997, Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing.

[39]  A. Treisman THE EFFECT OF IRRELEVANT MATERIAL ON THE EFFICIENCY OF SELECTIVE LISTENING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[40]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Oscillatory dynamics of response competition in human sensorimotor cortex , 2013, NeuroImage.

[43]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[44]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Frank,et al.  Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Activation During Response Competition , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  J. Duncan,et al.  Beyond the search surface: visual search and attentional engagement. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  Yaser Merrikhi,et al.  Visual attention: Linking prefrontal sources to neuronal and behavioral correlates , 2015, Progress in Neurobiology.

[49]  R. Nigbur,et al.  Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[50]  Tony W. Wilson,et al.  Is an absolute level of cortical beta suppression required for proper movement? Magnetoencephalographic evidence from healthy aging , 2016, NeuroImage.

[51]  Hakwan C. Lau,et al.  Dissociating response selection and conflict in the medial frontal surface , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  W. Drongelen,et al.  Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering , 1997, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[53]  Hedderik van Rijn,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms underlying individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2014, NeuroImage.

[54]  Alex I. Wiesman,et al.  Oscillatory dynamics and functional connectivity during gating of primary somatosensory responses , 2017, The Journal of physiology.

[55]  Scott A. Baldwin,et al.  Performance monitoring following conflict: Internal adjustments in cognitive control? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  A. Engel,et al.  Neuronal Synchronization along the Dorsal Visual Pathway Reflects the Focus of Spatial Attention , 2008, Neuron.

[57]  Bernhard Pastötter,et al.  Dynamic Adjustments of Cognitive Control: Oscillatory Correlates of the Conflict Adaptation Effect , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  J. Deutsch,et al.  Attention: Some theoretical considerations. , 1963 .